Skip to main content

Exploring the Challenges and Opportunities of Theory-Laden Observation and Subjectivity: A Key NOS Notion

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Nature of Science in Science Instruction

Part of the book series: Science: Philosophy, History and Education ((SPHE))

  • 1850 Accesses

Abstract

One key NOS aspect often recommended for inclusion in school science programs is the issue of subjectivity and bias (sometime called theory-laden observations) when viewing the world. Everyone has some expectations or prior “theories” when making observations and students should have opportunities to explore this notion and understand the role of theory-laden observations in science. The purpose of this chapter is to help teachers understand this issue both with discussion of the issue and an example. First, we explore the idea of subjectivity related to the way in which all humans—scientists included—engage in observing. In many cases, what observers expect to see can be helpful in allowing the observer to ignore things that are not important. In other cases, when observers expect to see something, they do. Second, this chapter reports the results of an experiment in which observers expected to see the heart of a tiny aquatic crustacean—daphnia—speed up or slow down when exposed to chemicals such as nicotine or alcohol. This experiment demonstrated the expectancy effect because, although students reported the anticipated results, there were no active chemicals involved but students believed that alcohol or nicotine was introduced to the daphnia.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 139.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Daphnia are a common type of freshwater crustacean sometimes called water fleas because of the hopping motion made when they swim. Their transparent shells allow students to see through to the internal organs. The small D. pulex has been available for many years, but the much larger D. magna has increased in popularity due to the enhanced ease with which students can see key structures.

References

  • Beveridge, W. I. B. (1957). The art of scientific investigation. New York: Norton.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, R. D. (1990). The effect of laboratory activity ordering on achievement and retention. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Southern Illinois University, Edwardsville.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boghai, D. (1978). A comparison of the effects of laboratory and discussion sequences on learning college chemistry. Dissertation Abstracts International, 39, 6045A.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boring, E. G. (1950). A history of experimental psychology (2nd ed.). New York: Appleton Century Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, W. (2015). Perception is theory-laden: The naturalized evidence and philosophical implications. Journal of general philosophy of science, 46, 121–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, W. F., & Lambert, B. L. (2001, September). The theory-ladenness of observation and the theory-ladenness of the rest of the scientific process. Philosophy of Science, 68(53), S176–S186. https://doi.org/10.1086/392907.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collette, A. T., & Chiapetta, E. L. (1994). Science instruction in the middle and secondary schools. New York: Macmillan Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cordaro, L., & Ison, J. R. (1963). Observer bias in classical conditioning of the planarian. Psychological Report, 13, 787–789.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. R. (1850). Darwin correspondence project (www.darwinproject.ac.uk). March 4 letter to Charles Henry Lardner Woodd (DAR 148:375).

  • Duschl, R. (1985). The changing concept of scientific observation. In R. Bybee (Ed.), NSTA yearbook: Science, technology and society (pp. 60–69). Arlington: National Science Teachers Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Franklin, A. (1981). Milliken’s published and unpublished data on oil drops. Historical Studies in the Physical Sciences, 11, 185–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Franklin, A. (2015). The theory-ladeness of experiment. Journal of general philosophy of science, 46, 155–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gould, S. J. (1994). The geometer of race. Discover, 15(11), 67–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, D. (1996). Using Daphnia to teach critical thinking about biological research. The American Biology Teacher, 58(3), 160–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hainsworth, M. (1956). The effect of previous knowledge on observation. The School Science Review, 37, 234–242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hainsworth, M. (1958). An experimental study of observation in school children. The School Science Review, 39, 264–276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hanson, N. R. (1958). Patterns of discovery. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, W. E. (1915). My wife and my mother-in-law. New York: Puck Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ivins, J. E. (1985). A comparison of the effects of two instructional sequences involving science laboratory activities. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH (ERIC Document Reproduction Service, No. ED 259 953).

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. L. (1942). Biology and training in observation. School Science Review, 23, 56–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lau, K., & Chan, S. (2013). Teaching about theory-laden observation to secondary students through manipulated lab inquiry experience. Science & Education, 22, 2641–2658.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lane, F. W. (1960). The kingdom of the octopus. New York: Sheriden.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leonard, W. H. (1980). Using an extended discretion approach in biology laboratory investigations. The American Biology Teacher, 42(7), 338–348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayr, E. (1991). One long argument. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McComas, W. F. (2005). Laboratory instruction in the service of science teaching and learning. The Science Teacher, 72(7), 24–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • McComas, W. F., & Moore, L. S. (2001). The expectancy effect in the secondary school laboratory: Issues and opportunities. American Biology Teacher, 63, 246–252.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nickerson, R. S. (1998, June), Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenomenon in many guises, Review of General Psychology, 2(2): 175–220. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.2.2.175.

  • Norris, S. (1984). Defining observational competence. Science Education, 68(2), 129–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norris, S. (1985). The philosophical basis of observation in science and science education. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 22(9), 817–833.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raghubir, K. P. (1979). The laboratory investigative approach to science instruction. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 16(1), 13–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R. (1976). Experimenter effects in behavioral research (Enlarged Edition). New York: Irvington Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R., & Fode, K. L. (1963). The effect of experimenter bias on the performance of the albino rat. Behavioral Research, 8, 183–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • SAPA (Science – A Process Approach). (1967). Washington, American Association for the Advancement of Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schindler, S. (2013). Theory-laden experimentation. Studies in the history and philosophy of science, 44, 89–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shapare, D. (1982). The concept of observation is science and philosophy. Philosophy of Science, 59, 485–525.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheldrake, R. (1995). Seven experiments that could change the world. New York: Riverbend Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tracy, G. (2018). Learning to see. American Scientist, 106(4), 242–249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Willson, V. L. (1987). Theory-building and theory-confirming observation in science and science-education. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 24(3), 279–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I thank the teachers, school administrators, and the hundreds of students who participated as subjects in this investigation and my original coauthor who provided much assistance with the experiment. This chapter has been heavily modified from McComas and Moore (2001) and is used with permission of the publisher of The American Biology Teacher.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to William F. McComas .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

A.Appendices

A.Appendices

1.1 Appendix A: The Use of Optical Illusions to Illustrate “Theory-Based” Observations

To illustrate the challenges inherent in “theory-based” observation, instructors may find it useful to secure a wide variety of optical illusions, particularly ones where two images are cleverly blended together in what are sometimes called ambiguous figures. One of these (Fig. 7.1) is the classic often referred to as Young Woman/Old Woman or My Wife and My Mother in Law. The origin of this figure is itself somewhat ambiguous with reports that it first appeared in a different form as a nineteenth century German postcard and was then redrawn by illustrator William Hill and published in the magazine Puck. Therefore, it seems reasonable to cite this as Hill (1915).

Fig. 7.1
An illustration of two clipart combined with an illusion of a young woman rotates her face to the right and an older woman's face in one picture.

Hill (1915). The classic My Wife and My Mother-in-Law ambiguous figure

For those who have not explored this image previously you will have no expectation. Some may first see a young woman who seems to be turning her face to the right. She is wearing a necklace and has a feather on her headscarf. It is just possible to see her left ear and chin, a bit of her left eyelash and her nose. With another look, the image may transform into an older woman who is turning her head somewhat to the left. The necklace of the young woman is now the woman’s mouth, the girl’s chin is now the nose of the older woman and what was previous the left ear becomes the older woman’s eye. They both share a while billowy headscarf, feather, and black hair.

To introduce the topic of “theory-based” observation to students not previously familiar with this image, a teacher could hand out cards to half the students saying, “Do you See the Old Woman in this Image?” and cards to the other half saying, “Do you See the Young Woman in this Image?” and then project the image to the entire class. Then ask the students to raise their hand if they see the Young Woman (or Old Woman, it doesn’t matter) and note which hands are raised. Certainly, there will be some who see the “wrong” image, have already seen this picture or can’t see either.

Generally, students who were “clued” by the statement on the card to look for the Old Woman will see that image most easily and vice versa. Having a range of such images and related optical illusions ready to share with the class along with two sets of cards directing students to look for different aspects of those images will provide some useful case material to engage students in a discussion of the role of prior expectations in observation. Fortunately, there are many such images available online for use in this fashion.

1.2 Appendix B: A Practical Example of Expectancy in Chemistry Class

In a series of lessons in chemistry class students explored some of the variables that affect the rate of gas production during electrolysis. One variable they noted was that the process occurs more quickly as the concentration of the electrolyte increased.

Next, I used this now-prior knowledge to reinforce the technique of electrolysis while introducing some of the problems of observation and thus discuss an important NOS element along with the traditional chemistry content. To extend this lesson and incorporate the expectancy effect, a bit of deceit was necessary.

I made a solution of sodium carbonate and poured that into three different containers. Each container was labeled a different concentration when in fact they were all the equal. I performed a demonstration for the students using a standard electrolysis set-up. I performed the demonstration three times, each with a supposedly different electrolyte solution and asked students to write down their observations. I mentioned the supposed concentration of electrolyte each time I did the demonstration and asked students simply to note anything of interest. They could have noted the time involved and the volume of gas produced if interested or simply watched from a qualitative perspective.

Students in each said things like “Oh wow look at how fast it’s going now!” However, the process was going at the same speed every time because the electrolyte concentration was identical in all three trials.

They knew from previous experiments that as the concentration of the electrolyte is increased the electrolysis rate also increases; so many students “saw” what they expected to see. Their expectations caused them to perceive an increase in reaction rate. Had they measured the amount of gas produced and noted the time involved they would have seen that all three trials produced the same amount of gas per unit time.

After the demonstration we had a conversation about the challenges of observation and the expectancy effect. Many of the students were amused that this could happen to them. Although I did hear one student remark, “I didn’t think anything special was happening, but everyone was saying it was, so I kept my opinion to myself.”

  • Contributed by Kent Woodard, Chemistry Teacher.

  • Rogers Arkansas Public Schools.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2020 The Editor(s) (if applicable) and The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

McComas, W.F. (2020). Exploring the Challenges and Opportunities of Theory-Laden Observation and Subjectivity: A Key NOS Notion. In: McComas, W.F. (eds) Nature of Science in Science Instruction. Science: Philosophy, History and Education. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-57239-6_7

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-57239-6_7

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-030-57238-9

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-030-57239-6

  • eBook Packages: EducationEducation (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics