Skip to main content

Bricoleurs and Theory-Building Qualitative Research: Responses to Responsiveness

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Respect for Thought

Part of the book series: Theory and History in the Human and Social Sciences ((THHSS))

Abstract

In his extended critique Smedslund suggested that scientific theory and research on psychotherapy are not feasible because (1) people respond to myriad, constantly shifting determinants, and their behavior, (2) evolves in ever-compounding sequences that are not precisely predictable, (3) is never precisely repeated, and (4) is deeply enmeshed in interactions with other people. Instead, he suggests that therapists are and should be bricoleurs, drawing on psychological common sense, which includes knowledge shared by virtue of being human, understandings acquired though language and culture, and personal familiarity with the client.

Smedslund’s critique unpacks what I have previously characterized as appropriate responsiveness: therapists strive to do the right thing in response to ever-changing client requirements and emerging context. Despite his cogent critique, however, I am more optimistic than Smedslund about theory and research on psychotherapy.A key distinction is between explanatory theories and treatment theories. An explanatory theory describes what things are and how they are related to each other within a domain. They are evaluated by comparing detailed observations with theoretical tenets and derivations. I call this theory-building research. A treatment theory describes the principles and practices that guide clinicians in conducting a therapy. They propose to say what works, and they tell therapists what to do. They are evaluated by assessing whether the treatment is effective. I call this product-testing research, illustrated by clinical trials. I argue that Smedslund was on target for treatment theory and product-testing research but that qualitative theory-building research on explanatory theory offers a strategy that can address Smedslund’s critique.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Many descriptive process measures have been developed (e.g., Brauner et al. 2018; Worthington & Bodie 2018). However, these are generally not statistically associated with the evaluative outcome measures, presumably because therapists use them responsively, and hence approximately optimally, so they contribute negligibly to the prediction of outcome (Stiles 1988, 2013; Stiles et al. 1986, 1998). That is, because of responsiveness, statistical association of a process component with client improvement tends to be unrelated to the clinical importance of that component in producing that improvement. Nevertheless, because descriptive process measures have not predicted outcome, they have tended to be discounted.

References

  • Basto, I., Stiles, W. B., Bento, T., Pinheiro, P., Mendes, I., Rijo, D., & Salgado, J. (2018). Fluctuation in the assimilation of problematic experiences: A case study of dynamic systems analysis. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1–10. [1119]. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brauner, E., Boos, M., & Kolbe, M. (Eds.). (2018). Cambridge handbook of group interaction analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brinegar, M. G., Salvi, L. M., Stiles, W. B., & Greenberg, L. S. (2006). Building a meaning bridge: Therapeutic progress from problem formulation to understanding. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 53, 165–180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, D. T. (1957). Factors relevant to the validity of experiments in social settings. Psychological Bulletin, 54, 297–312. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0040950.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, D. T. (1979). “Degrees of freedom” and the case study. In T. D. Cook & C. S. Reichardt (Eds.), Qualitative and quantitative methods in evaluation research (pp. 49–67). Beverley Hills, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, T. A., & Stiles, W. B. (2016). Some problems with randomized controlled trials and some viable alternatives. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 23, 87–95. https://doi.org/10.1002/cpp.1942.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Caro Gabalda, I., & Stiles, W. B. (2013). Irregular assimilation progress: Reasons for setbacks in the context of linguistic therapy of evaluation. Psychotherapy Research, 23, 35–53.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Castonguay, L., & Hill, C. E. (Eds.). (2018). How and why are some therapists better than others? Understanding therapist effects (pp. 71–84). Washington, DC: APA Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dyson, F. W., Eddington, A. S., & Davidson, C. R. (1920). A determination of the deflection of light by the sun’s gravitational field, from observations made at the solar eclipse of May 29, 1919. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society A, 220, 571–581.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hardy, G. E., Stiles, W. B., Barkham, M., & Startup, M. (1998). Therapist responsiveness to client interpersonal styles during time-limited treatments for depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 304–312.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Honos-Webb, L., Stiles, W. B., Greenberg, L. S., & Goldman, R. (1998). Assimilation analysis of process-experiential psychotherapy: A comparison of two cases. Psychotherapy Research, 8, 264–286.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kramer, U., & Stiles, W. B. (2015). The responsiveness problem in psychotherapy: A review of proposed solutions. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 22, 277–295.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. S. (1970). The structure of scientific revolutions (2nd ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lambert, M. J. (2013). The efficacy and effectiveness of psychotherapy. In M. J. Lambert (Ed.), Bergin & Garfield’s handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (6th ed., pp. 169–218). New York, NY: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leiman, M., & Stiles, W. B. (2001). Dialogical sequence analysis and the zone of proximal development as conceptual enhancements to the assimilation model: The case of Jan revisited. Psychotherapy Research, 11, 311–330.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levins, R. (1968). Evolution in changing environments: Some theoretical explorations. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levi-Strauss, C. (1966). The savage mind. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luborsky, L., & DeRubeis, R. J. (1984). The use of psychotherapy treatment manuals: A small revolution in psychotherapy research style. Clinical Psychology Review, 4, 5–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luborsky, L., Singer, B., & Luborsky, L. (1975). Comparative studies of psychotherapies: Is it true that “everyone has won and all must have prizes”? Archives of General Psychiatry, 32, 995–1008.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Norcross, J. C. (Ed.). (2002). Psychotherapy relationships that work: Therapist contributions and responsiveness to patient need. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norcross, J. C. (Ed.). (2011). Psychotherapy relationships that work: Evidence-based responsiveness (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peirce, C. S. (1985). The proper treatment of hypotheses: A preliminary chapter, toward an examination of Hume’s argument against miracles, in its logic and in its history. In C. Eisele (Ed.), Historical perspectives on Peirce’s logic of science. A history of science (Vol. 2). Berlin: Mouton Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rennie, D. L. (2012). Qualitative research as methodical hermeneutics. Psychological Methods, 17, 385–398. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029250.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, C. R. (1959). A theory of therapy, personality, and interpersonal relationships as developed by the client-centered framework. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a science (Formulations of a person and the social context) (Vol. Vol. III, pp. 184–256). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenzweig, S. (1936). Some implicit common factors in diverse methods of psychotherapy. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 6, 412–415.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salvatore, S., & Valsiner, J. (2010). Between the general and the unique: Overcoming the nomothetic versus idiographic opposition. Theory & Psychology, 20, 817–833.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smedslund, J. (2009). The mismatch between current research methods and the nature of psychological phenomena. Theory & Psychology, 19, 778–794.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smedslund, J. (2012a). The bricoleur model of psychological practice. Theory & Psychology, 22, 643–657.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smedslund, J. (2012b). What follows from what we all know about human beings? Theory & Psychology, 22, 658–668.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smedslund, J. (2012c). Psycho-logic: Some thoughts and after-thoughts. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 53, 295–302.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smedslund, J. (2015). The value of experiments in psychology. In J. Martin, J. Sugarman, & K. L. Slaney (Eds.), The Wiley handbook of theoretical and philosophical psychology (pp. 359–373). Oxford: Wiley Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smedslund, J. (2016a). Practicing psychology without an empirical evidence-base: The bricoleur model. New Ideas in Psychology, 43, 50–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smedslund, J. (2016b). Why psychology cannot be an empirical science. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 50, 185–195.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stiles, W. B. (1983). Normality, diversity, and psychotherapy. Psychotherapy, 20, 183–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiles, W. B. (1988). Psychotherapy process-outcome correlations may be misleading. Psychotherapy, 25, 27–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiles, W. B. (1992). Producers and consumers of psychotherapy research ideas. Journal of Psychotherapy Practice and Research, 1, 305–307.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiles, W. B. (2001). Assimilation of problematic experiences. Psychotherapy, 38, 462–465.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiles, W. B. (2006). Numbers can be enriching. New Ideas in Psychology, 24, 252–262.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiles, W. B. (2007). Theory-building case studies of counselling and psychotherapy. Counselling and Psychotherapy Research, 7, 122–127.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiles, W. B. (2009a). Logical operations in theory-building case studies. Pragmatic Case Studies in Psychotherapy, 5(3), 9–22. https://doi.org/10.14713/pcsp.v5i3.973. Available: http://jrul.libraries.rutgers.edu/index.php/pcsp/article/view/973.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stiles, W. B. (2009b). Responsiveness as an obstacle for psychotherapy outcome research: It’s worse than you think. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 16, 86–91.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiles, W. B. (2011). Coming to terms. Psychotherapy Research, 21, 367–384. https://doi.org/10.1080/10503307.2011.582186.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stiles, W. B. (2013). The variables problem and progress in psychotherapy research. Psychotherapy, 50, 33–41.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stiles, W. B. (2017). Theory-building case studies. In D. Murphy (Ed.), Counselling Psychology: A Textbook for Study and Practice (pp. 439-452). Chichester, UK: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiles, W. B. (2018). Assimilation of problematic voices and the historicity of signs: How culture enters psychotherapy. In A. Konopka, H. J. M. Hermans, & M. M. Goncalves (Eds.), Handbook of dialogical self theory and psychotherapy: Bridging psychotherapeutic and cultural traditions (pp. 56–72). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiles, W. B. (in press). Responsiveness in psychotherapy research: Problems and ways forward. In J. C. Watson & H. Wiseman (Eds), Attuning to enhance therapist responsiveness. Washington, DC: APA Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiles, W. B., Elliott, R., Llewelyn, S. P., Firth-Cozens, J. A., Margison, F. R., Shapiro, D. A., & Hardy, G. (1990). Assimilation of problematic experiences by clients in psychotherapy. Psychotherapy, 27, 411–420.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiles, W. B., Hill, C. E., & Elliott, R. (2015). Looking both ways. Psychotherapy Research, 25, 282–293. https://doi.org/10.1080/10503307.2014.981681.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stiles, W. B., Honos-Webb, L., & Surko, M. (1998). Responsiveness in psychotherapy. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 5, 439–458.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiles, W. B., & Horvath, A. O. (2017). Appropriate responsiveness as a contribution to therapist effects. In L. Castonguay & C. E. Hill (Eds.), How and why are some therapists better than others? Understanding therapist effects (pp. 71–84). Washington, DC: APA Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiles, W. B., Morrison, L. A., Haw, S. K., Harper, H., Shapiro, D. A., & Firth-Cozens, J. (1991). Longitudinal study of assimilation in exploratory psychotherapy. Psychotherapy, 28, 195–206.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiles, W. B., Shapiro, D. A., & Elliott, R. (1986). Are all psychotherapies equivalent? American Psychologist, 41, 165–180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiles, W. B., & Wolfe, B. E. (2006). Relationship factors in treating anxiety disorders. In L. G. Castonguay & L. E. Beutler (Eds.), Principles of therapeutic change that work (pp. 155–165). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J., & Brinkmann, S. (2016). Beyond the “variables”: Developing metalanguage for psychology. In S. H. Klempe & R. Smith (Eds.), Centrality of history for theory construction in psychology (Annals of theoretical psychology) (Vol. 14, pp. 75–90).

    Google Scholar 

  • Wampold, B. E., & Imel, Z. E. (2015). The great psychotherapy debate: The evidence for what makes psychotherapy work (2nd ed.). London: Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Worthington, D. L., & Bodie, G. D. (Eds.). (2018). Sourcebook of listening research methodology and measures (pp. 605–611). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to William B. Stiles .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2020 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Stiles, W.B. (2020). Bricoleurs and Theory-Building Qualitative Research: Responses to Responsiveness. In: Lindstad, T., Stänicke, E., Valsiner, J. (eds) Respect for Thought. Theory and History in the Human and Social Sciences. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-43066-5_20

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics