Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

How Does Male Ritual Behavior Vary Across the Lifespan?

An Examination of Fijian Kava Ceremonies

  • Published:
Human Nature Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Ritual behaviors of some form exist in every society known to anthropologists. Despite this universality, we have little understanding of how ritual behavior varies within populations or across the lifespan, nor the determinants of this variation. Here we test hypotheses derived from life history theory by using behavioral observations and oral interview data concerning participant variation in Fijian kava-drinking ceremonies. We predicted that substantial variation in the frequency and duration of participation would result from (1) trade-offs with reproduction and (2) the intrinsic status differences between ritual participants. We demonstrate that when controlling for household composition, men with young offspring participated less frequently and exhibited greater variance in their time spent at ceremonies than men without young children. However, men with a larger number of total dependents in their household participated more frequently than those with fewer. Moreover, we found that men’s ascribed rank, level of education, and reliance on wage labor all significantly predict their frequency of attendance. We also found that the number of dependents a man has in his household is positively correlated with total food production, and the amount of kava he cultivates. In general, these results suggest that ritual participation is part of an important strategy employed by Fijian men for both achieving status and developing social alliances. Variation in participation in kava ceremonies by Fijian men therefore reflects the constraints of their current life history condition and their inherited rank.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Both Nayacakalou (1955) and France (1969) note that this idealized structure was imposed upon the peoples of Fiji by the British colonial administration in order to rationalize and record land ownership. Prior to the arrival of colonial officials, social structure was highly variable across Fiji, and although some regional variation exists, the system described by Nayacakalou mirrors informant descriptions from the current study population.

  2. There is likely an optimal level of kanikani signaling. If a man has high levels of kanikani but largely shirks his work and/or family responsibilities, he will not realize these cooperative benefits. Rather, men with kanikani must also work hard and invest in their family in order to realize these benefits.

References

  • Alcorta, C. S. (2006). Religion and the life course: Is adolescence an “experience expectant” period for religious transmission? In P. McNamara (Ed.), Where God and science meet: How brain and evolutionary studies alter our understanding of religion, Vol. 1: Evolution, genes, and the religious brain (pp. 55–79). Westport: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alcorta, C., & Sosis, R. (2005). Ritual, emotion, and sacred symbols: the evolution of religion as an adaptive complex. Human Nature, 16, 323–359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, Q., & Whitehouse, H. (2011). The cultural morphospace of ritual forms: examining modes of religiosity cross-culturally. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32, 50–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barker, J. (1992). Christianity in western Melanesian ethnography. In J. Carrier (Ed.), History and tradition in Melanesian anthropology (pp. 144–173). Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bock, J. (2002). Learning, life history, and productivity: children’s lives in the Okavango Delta of Botswana. Human Nature, 13(2), 161–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bock, J., & Johnson, S. E. (2004). Subsistence ecology and play among the Okavango Delta peoples of Botswana. Human Nature, 15(1), 63–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brewster, A. B. (1922). The hill tribes of Fiji. Philadelphia: J.P. Lippincott.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brison, K. (2007). Our wealth is loving each other: Self and society in Fiji. Lanham: Lexington Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, D. (1991). Human universals. Boston: McGraw Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brunton, R. (1989). The abandoned narcotic: Kava and cultural instability in Melanesia. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burt, B. (1994). Tradition and Christianity: The colonial transformation of a Solomon Island society. London: Harwood.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cairney, S., Maruff, P., & Cough, A. (2002). The neurobehavioural effects of kava. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 36, 675–682.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cairney, S., Clough, A., Maruff, P., Collie, A., & Currie, B. (2003). Saccade and cognitive function in chronic kava users. Neuropsychopharmacology, 28(2), 389–396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cawte, J. (1986). Parameters of kava used as a challenge to alcohol. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 20, 70–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charnov, E. (1993). Life history invariants. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cronk, L. (1994). Evolutionary theories of morality and the manipulative use of signals. Zygon Journal of Religion and Science, 29(1), 81–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Aguilar, R., & Cronk, L. (2011). Stratification and supernatural punishment: cooperation or obedience? Religion, Brain & Behavior, 1(1), 73–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • France, P. (1969). The charter of the land: Custom and colonization in Fiji. Melbourne: Oxford University Press.

  • Galdikas, B., & Wood, J. (1990). Birth spacing patterns in humans and apes. Journal of Physical Anthropology, 83, 185–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ginges, J., Hansen, I. G., & Norenzayan, A. (2009). Religion and popular support for suicide attacks. Psychological Science, 20, 224–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guo, P. (2009). Separate space, negotiating power: Dynamics of ancestral spirits and Christian god in Langalanga. In P. Stewart & A. Strathern (Eds.), Religious and ritual change: Cosmologies and histories (pp. 69–108). Durham: Carolina Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gurven, M., & Kaplan, H. (2006). Determinants of time allocation to production across the lifespan among the Machiguenga and Piro Indians of Peru. Human Nature, 17(1), 1–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gurven, M., Kaplan, H., & Gutierrez, M. (2006). How long does it take to become a proficient hunter? Implications for the evolution of delayed growth. Journal of Human Evolution, 51, 454–470.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hames, R. (1992). Time allocation. In E. A. Smith & B. Winterhalder (Eds.), Evolutionary ecology and human behavior (pp. 203–236). New York: Aldine de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J. F., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (1997). Hadza women’s time allocation, offspring provisioning, and the evolution of long postmenopausal life spans. Current Anthropology, 38(4), 551–577.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, K., & Kaplan, H. (1999). Life history traits in humans: theory and empirical studies. Annual Review of Anthropology, 28, 397–430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hocart, A. M. (1929). Lau Islands, Fiji. Honolulu: Bernice P. Bishop Museum, Bulletin 62.

  • Irons, W. (1979). Cultural and biological success. In N. A. Chagnon & W. Irons (Eds.), Evolutionary biology and human social behavior: An anthropological perspective (pp. 257–272). North Scituate: Duxbury Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Irons, W. (2001). Religion as a hard to fake sign of commitment. In R. M. Neese (Ed.), Evolution and the capacity for commitment (pp. 292–309). New York: Russell Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, M. (1990). Christianity, people of the land, and chiefs in Fiji. In J. Barker (Ed.), Christianity in Oceania: Ethnographic perspectives (pp. 127–147). Lanham: University Press of America.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, H. (1994). Evolutionary and wealth flows theories of fertility: empirical tests and new models. Population and Development Review, 20(4), 753–791.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, M. (1995). Neither cargo nor cult: Ritual politics and the colonial imagination in Fiji. Durham: Duke University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, H. (1996). A theory of fertility and parental investment in traditional and modern societies. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology, 39, 91–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, H., & Bock, J. (2001) Fertility theory: The embodied capital theory of life history evolution. In J. Hoem (Ed.) The international encyclopedia of the social and behavioral sciences (pp. 5561–5568). Elsevier.

  • Kaplan, H., Lancaster, J. B., Bock, J. A., & Johnson, S. E. (1995). Does observed fertility maximize fitness among New Mexican men? A test of an optimality model and a new theory of parental investment in the embodied capital of offspring. Human Nature, 6, 325–360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, H., Hill, K., Lancaster, J., & Hurtado, A. M. (2000). A theory of human life history evolution: diet, intelligence, and longevity. Evolutionary Anthropology, 9, 156–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirch, P. (1984). The evolution of Polynesian chiefdoms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuzawa, C., & Bragg, J. (2012). Plasticity in human life history strategy: implications for contemporary human variation and the evolution of the genus homo. Current Anthropology, 53(S6), S369–S382.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lebot, V. (1995). The origin and distribution of kava. Canberra Anthropology, 18, 20–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lebot, V., Merlin, M., & Lindstrom, L. (1992). Kava: The Pacific elixir. Rochester: Healing Arts Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leinard, P. (2011). Life-stages and risk avoidance: status and context-sensitivity in precaution systems. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 1067–1074.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levins, R. (1968). Evolution in changing environments. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marlowe, F. (2003). A critical period for provisioning by Hadza men: implications for pair bonding. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 217–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McKay, R., Herold, J., & Whitehouse, H. (2013). Catholic guilt? Recall of confession promotes prosocial behavior. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 3, 201–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nayacakalou, R. R. (1955). The Fijian system of kinship and marriage. Journal of the Polynesian Society, 64, 44–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norenzayan, A., & Shariff, A. F. (2008). The origin and evolution of religious prosociality. Science, 322, 58–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norton, S., & Ruze, P. (1994). Kava dermopathy. Journal of the American Academy of Dermatology, 31, 89–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raven-Hart, R. (1956). A village in the Yasawas (Fiji). Journal of the Polynesian Society, 65(2), 95–154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roff, D. A. (1992). The evolution of life histories. London: Chapman and Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruffle, B. J., & Sosis, R. (2007). Does it pay to pray costly rituals and cooperation. The B.E. Press of Economic Policy and Analysis (Contributions), 7, 1–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryle, J. (2010). My God, my land: Interwoven paths of Christianity and tradition in Fiji. Farnham: Ashgate.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sahlins, M. (1987). Islands of history. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shaver, J. (2012). The behavioral ecology of Fijian religion. Unpublished PhD dissertation, Department of Anthropology, University of Connecticut, Storrs.

  • Singh, Y., & Singh, N. (2002). Therapeutic potential of kava in the treatment of anxiety disorders. CNS Drugs, 16(11), 731–743.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Soler, M. (2012). Costly signaling, ritual, and cooperation: evidence from Candomble an Afro-Brazilian religion. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33, 346–356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sosis, R. (2005). Does religion promote trust? The role of signaling, reputation, and punishment. Interdisciplinary Journal of Research on Religion, 1, 1–30 (Article 7).

    Google Scholar 

  • Sosis, R. (2006). Religious behaviors, badges, and bans: Signaling theory and the evolution of religion. In P. McNamara (Ed.), Where god and science meet: How brain and evolutionary studies alter our understanding of religion, volume 1: Evolution, genes, and the religious brain (pp. 61–86). Westport: Praeger Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sosis, R., & Alcorta, C. S. (2003). Signaling, solidarity, and the sacred: the evolution of religious behavior. Evolutionary Anthropology, 12, 264–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sosis, R., & Bressler, E. (2003). Cooperation and commune longevity: a test of the costly signaling theory of religion. Cross-Cultural Research, 37, 211–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sosis, R., & Bulbulia, J. (2011). The behavioral ecology of religion: the benefits and costs of one evolutionary approach. Religion, 41(3), 341–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stearns, S. (1992). The evolution of life histories. Oxford University Press.

  • Strathern, P., & Strathern, A. (2009). Religious and ritual change: Cosmologies and histories. Durham: Carolina Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, G. E. (1960). The birth of the gods: The origin of primitive beliefs. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomson, B. (1908). The Fijians: A study of the decay of custom. London: William Heinemann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thornley, A. W. (2005). A shaking of the land: William Cross and the origins of Christianity in Fiji. Ko Wiliame Korosi kei na i tekitekivu ni lotu Vakaristo e Viti. Suva: institute of Pacific Studies.

  • Tomlinson, M. (2006). Retheorizing mana: Bible translation and discourse of loss in Fiji. Oceania, 76(2), 173–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomlinson, M. (2007). Everything and its opposite: Kava drinking in Fiji. Anthropological Quarterly, 80(4), 1065–1081.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomlinson, M. (2009). In God’s image: The metaculture of Christianity in Fiji. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toren, C. (1990). Making sense of hierarchy: Cognition as social process in Fiji. London: London School of Economics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toren, C. (1994). The drinker as chief or rebel: Kava and alcohol in Fiji. In M. McDonald (Ed.), Gender, drink and drugs (pp. 153–173). Oxford: Berg.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner, J. (1986). “The water of life”: Kava ritual and the logic of sacrifice. Ethnology, 25(3), 203–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turner, J. (1992). Ritual, habitus, and hierarchy in Fiji. Ethnology, 31(4), 219–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turner, J. (1995). Substance, symbol, and practice: the power of kava in Fijian society. Canberra Anthropology, 18(1), 87–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Gennep, A. (2004 [1904]). The rites of passage. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

  • von Rueden, C., Gurven, M., & Kaplan, H. (2011). Why do men seek high social status? Fitness payoffs to dominance and prestige. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 278, 2223–2232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walker, R., Hill, K., & McMillen, G. (2002). Age-dependency in skill, strength and hunting ability among the Ache of Eastern Paraguay. Journal of Human Evolution, 42, 639–657.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walker, R., Gurven, M., Burger, O., & Hamilton, M. (2008). The trade-off between number and size of offspring in humans and other primates. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 275, 827–834.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waterhouse, J. (1997 [1866]). The king and the people of Fiji. Reprint. Auckland: Pasifika Press.

  • Weisfeld, G. (1997). Puberty rites as clues to the nature of human adolescence. Cross-Cultural Research: The Journal of Comparative Social Science, 31, 27–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wrangham, R. W., & Peterson, D. (1996). Demonic males: Apes and the origins of human violence. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Xygalatas, D. (2013). Effects of religious setting on cooperative behavior: a case study from Mauritius. Religion, Brain, and Behavior, 3(2), 91–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Xygalatas, D., Mitkidis, P., Fischer, R., Reddish, P., Skewes, J., Geertz, A. W., et al. (2013). Extreme rituals promote prosociality. Psychological Science, 24(8), 1602–1605.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank and acknowledge support from the University of Connecticut Department of Anthropology, Oxford University’s Cognition, Religion and Theology Project which was funded by the Templeton Foundation, National Science Foundation Doctoral Dissertation Improvement Grant # 1023456, Center of Theological Inquiry Fellowship on Evolution and Human Nature, and European Social Science Research Council Large Grant (REF RES-060-25-0085) entitled “Ritual, Community, and Conflict.” We thank Candace Alcorta, Phil Glauberman, Jordan Kiper, Ben Purzycki, and anonymous reviewers for comments on earlier drafts of this paper, and Talatala Taniela Vakacegu and Ironi Vunisea for their generosity, kindness, support, and friendship.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to John H. Shaver.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Shaver, J.H., Sosis, R. How Does Male Ritual Behavior Vary Across the Lifespan?. Hum Nat 25, 136–160 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-014-9191-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-014-9191-6

Keywords

Navigation