Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

The Influence of Improper Information on Japanese Lay Judges’ Determination of Punishment

  • Published:
Asian Journal of Criminology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Lay judges’ decision-making process in the determination of appropriate punishment appears to be based on their assessment of the seriousness of the crime; this indicates that legal sentences are being decided subjectively, which can be problematic. For instance, judgments can sometimes be made on the basis of irrelevant and therefore improper, information. Ideally, such information would be disregarded when considering appropriate punishment in a real court. Thus, the aim of this study was to determine whether information that is irrelevant to a criminal case can influence Japanese lay judges’ determination of an appropriate punishment. Our study used a fictional case study and a questionnaire to assess whether Japanese participants were influenced by exposure to irrelevant information. The results of two experiments consistently showed that irrelevant information distorts the inner subjective balance between the severity of punishment and the seriousness of the crime and may even influence the decision-making process that lay judges engage in when determining offenders’ punishment. We suggest reasons why this influence might affect decision-making and discuss whether the influence of improper information can be consciously disregarded.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alter, A. L., Kernochan, J., & Darley, J. M. (2007). Transgression wrongfulness outweighs its harmfulness as a determinant of sentence severity. Law and Human Behavior, 31, 319–335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, B. H. (1994). David, Goliath, and Reverend Bayes: prior beliefs about defendants’ status in personal injury cases. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 8, 233–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, B. (1999). The ecological validity of jury simulations: is the jury still out? Law and Human Behavior, 23, 75–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlsmith, K. M. (2006). The roles of retribution and utility in determining punishment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 437–451.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlsmith, K. M., Darley, J. M., & Robinson, P. H. (2002). Why do we punish? Deterrence and just deserts as motives for punishment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 284–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cather, C., Greene, E., & Durham, R. (1996). Plaintiff injury and defendant reprehensibility: implications for compensatory and punitive damages. Law and Human Behavior, 20, 189–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chaiken, S., & Maheswaran, D. (1994). Heuristic processing can bias systematic processing: effects of source credibility, argument ambiguity, and task importance on attitude judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(3), 460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Darley, J. M., Carlsmith, K. M., & Robinson, P. H. (2000). Incapacitation and just deserts as motives for punishment. Law and Human Behavior, 24, 659–683.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erickson, M. L., & Gibbs, J. P. (1979). On the perceived severity of legal penalties. The Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 70, 102–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, E., Johns, M., & Bowman, J. (1999). The effects of injury severity on jury negligence decisions. Law and Human Behavior, 23, 675–693.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hammitt, J. K., Carroll, S. J., & Relles, D. A. (1985). Tort standards and jury decisions. The Journal of Legal Studies, 14, 751–762.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hans, V. P., & Ermann, M. D. (1989). Responses to corporate versus individual wrongdoing. Law and Human Behavior, 13, 151–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harley, E. M. (2007). Hindsight bias in legal decision-making. Social Cognition, 25, 48–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hastie, R., Schkade, D. A., & Payne, J. W. (1999). Juror judgments in civil cases: hindsight effects on judgments of liability for punitive damages. Law and Human Behavior, 23, 597–614.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Igou, E., & Bless, H. (2005). The conversational basis for the dilution effect. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 24, 25–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kolber, A. J. (2009). The comparative nature of punishment. Boston University Law Review, 89, 1565–1608.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R. E., Zukier, H., & Lemley, R. E. (1981). The dilution effect: nondiagnostic information weakens the implications of diagnostic information. Cognitive Psychology, 13, 248–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Penrod, S. D., & Cutler, B. L. (1995). Witness confidence and witness accuracy: assessing their forensic relation. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 1, 817–845.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robbennolt, J. (2002). Punitive damage decision-making: the decisions of citizens and trial court judges. Law and Human Behavior, 26, 315–341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rossi, P. H., Waite, E., Bose, C. E., & Berk, R. E. (1974). The seriousness of crimes: normative structure and individual differences. American Sociological Review, 39, 224–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution and judgement of well-being: informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 513–523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, A. C., & Greene, E. (2005). Conduct and its consequences: attempts at debiasing jury judgments. Law and Human Behavior, 29, 505–526.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sporer, S. L., & Goodman-Delahunty, J. (2009). Disparities in sentencing decisions. Social psychology of punishment of crime. In M. E. Oswald, S. Bieneck, & J. Hupfeld-Heinemann (eds.), Social psychology of punishment of crime (pp. 379–401). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warr, M. (1989). What is the perceived seriousness of crimes? Criminology, 27, 795–821.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Warr, M., Meier, R. F., & Erickson, M. L. (1983). Norms, theories of punishment, and publicly preferred penalties for crimes. The Sociological Quarterly, 24, 75–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G. L., & Quinlivan, D. S. (2009). The eyewitness post–identification feedback effect: what is the function of flexible confidence estimates for autobiographical events? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23, 1153–1163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wrightsman, L. S. (1999). Judicial decision-making: is psychology relevant? New York: Kluwer/Plenum Publishers.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Zukier, H. (1982). The dilution effect: the role of correlation and the dispersion of predictor variables in the use of nondiagnostic information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 1163–1174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported in part by grants from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (grant number: 21 9821).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Eiichiro Watamura.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Watamura, E., Wakebe, T. & Karasawa, K. The Influence of Improper Information on Japanese Lay Judges’ Determination of Punishment. Asian Criminology 9, 285–300 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11417-014-9193-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11417-014-9193-7

Keywords

Navigation