Skip to main content
Log in

How does voice matter? Evidence from the ultimatum game

  • Published:
Experimental Economics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Prior research has demonstrated that the ability to express one’s views or “voice” matters in social and economic interactions, but little is known of the mechanisms through which voice operates. Using an experimental approach based on the ultimatum game with the strategy method, we explore four potential channels for voice that encompass and expand on prior work: the knowledge effect of voice, the value expressive (or inherent value) of voice, the expectation effect of voice, and the procedural fairness effects of voice. Our results show strong effects through the value expressive and expectation channel, but not through either the knowledge channel or procedural fairness. In our view, voice is powerful because people like to express their views and they are disappointed when their views did not make a difference in their outcomes.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. To test the sensitivity of our findings, we adopted a more stringent criterion to determine the highest ranked category for each message. A message was classified under a category only if there was more than 75% of the agreement among message evaluators. With this criterion, we found that “fairness” was still the leading motivation behind most messages. 59% of the responders in the voice treatment and 52% of the responders from the VE treatment wrote messages to express fairness.

  2. Another way of showing that intention matters is by comparing the MAOs of responders who requested a fair offer but instead were given a low offer with the MAOs of responders who did not request a fair offer and were given a low offer. If intention matters, the MAOs of the former should be higher than the MAOs of the latter. This is because the low offer made by proposers might be labeled as something that is more negative when responders requested a fair offer than when they did not request it. Unfortunately, we do not have enough observations of these two groups of responders to make a meaningful statistical comparison.

  3. Proposers in no voice treatment and voice to experimenter treatment received the same instructions, which is that their matched responders were not randomly selected to send a message to them regarding their desired offers.

References

  • Brandts, J., & Charness, G. (2011). The strategy versus the direct-response method: a first survey of experimental comparisions. Experimental Economics, 14(3), 375–398.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bolton, G., & Ockenfels, A. (2000). ERC: a theory of equity, reciprocity, and competition. American Economic Review, 90(1), 166–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burke, K., & Leben, S. (2007). Procedural fairness: a key ingredient in public satisfaction. Court Review, 44(1–2), 4–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Camerer, C. (2003). Behavioral game theory: experiments in strategic interaction. Roundtable series in behavioral economics. Princeton/New York: Princeton University Press/Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crawford, V. (1998). A survey of experiments on communication via cheap talk. Journal of Economic Theory, 78(2), 286–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dolan, P., Edlin, R., Tsuchiya, A., & Wailoo, A. (2007). It ain’t what you do, it’s the way that you do it: characteristics of procedural justice and their importance in social decision-making. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 64, 157–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, E., & Schmidt, K. (1999). A Theory of fairness, competition, and cooperation. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 114(3), 817–868.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Folger, R. (1977). Distributive and procedural justice: combined impact of “voice” and improvement of experienced inequity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 108–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, J., & Folger, R. (1983). Procedural justice, participation and the fair process effect in groups and organizations. In P. B. Paulus (Ed.), Basic group processes (pp. 235–256). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, J. (1990). Organizational justice: yesterday, today and tomorrow. Journal of Management, 16, 399–432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Habermas, J. (1970). Technology and science as ‘ideology’. Towards a Rational Society. Boston: Beacon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirschman, A. (1970). Exit, voice, and loyalty: responses to decline in firms, organizations, and slates. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, E., McCabe, K., & Smith, V. (1996). On expectations and the monetary stakes in ultimatum games. International Journal of Game Theory, 25(3), 289–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Houser, D., & Xiao, E. (2010). Classification of natural language messages using a coordination game. Experimental Economics, 14, 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., Knetsch, J., & Thaler, R. (1986). Fairness and the assumptions of economics. Journal of Business, 59, 285–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., Knetsch, J., & Thaler, R. (1991). Anomalies: the endowment effect, loss aversion and status quo bias. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 5(1), 193–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Katz, D. (1960). The functional approach to the study of attitudes. Public Opinion Quarterly, 24, 163–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Köszegi, B., & Rabin, M. (2006). A model of reference-dependent preferences. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 121(4), 1133–1165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lind, E., Kurtz, S., Musante, L., Walker, L., & Thibaut, J. (1980). Procedural and outcome effects on reactions to adjudicated resolution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 643–653.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lind, E., Kanfer, R., & Earley, P. (1990). Voice, control and procedural justice: instrumental and no instrumental concerns in fairness judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 952–959.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lind, E., Kulik, C., Ambrose, M., & De Vera Park, M. (1993). Individual and corporate dispute resolution: Using procedural fairness as a decision heuristic. Administrative Science Quarterly, 38, 224–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • List, J., & Cherry, T. (2000). Learning to accept in the ultimatum game: evidence from an experimental design that generates low offers. Experimental Economics, 3(1), 11–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miceli, M. P., & Lane, M. C. (1991). Antecedents of pay satisfaction: a review and extension. In K. Rowland & G. R. Ferris (Eds.), Research in personnel and human resources management (pp. 235–309). Greenwich: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, W. (2002). Equity or intention: it’s the thought that counts. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 48, 423–430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, R. (1999). Can you have too much of a good thing? The limits of voice for improving satisfaction with leaders. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 313–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, A., & Erev, I. (1995). Learning in extensive-form games: experimental data and simple dynamic models in the intermediate term. Games and Economic Behavior, 8, 164–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Selten, R. (1967). Die Strategiemethode zur Erforschung des eingeschränkt rationale Verhaltens im Rahmen eines Oligopolexperiments. In H. Sauermann (Ed.), Beiträge zur experimentellen Wirtschaftsforschung (pp. 136–168). Tübingen: Mohr.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thaler, R. (1988). Anomalies: the ultimatum game. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 2, 195–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thibaut, J., & Walker, L. (1975). Procedural justice: a psychological analysis. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, T., Rasinski, K., & Spodick, N. (1985). The influence of voice on satisfaction with leaders: exploring the meaning of process control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 72–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, T. (1987). Conditions leading to value expressive effects in judgments of procedural justice: a test of four models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 333–344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, T., & Blader, S. (2003). Procedural Justice, social identity, and cooperative behaviour. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 7, 349–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van den Bos, K. (1999). What are we talking about when we talk about no-voice procedures? On the psychology of the fair outcome effect. Journal of Experimental and Social Psychology, 35, 560–577.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vidmar, N. (1992). Procedural fairness and alternative dispute resolution. Psychological Science, 3(4), 224–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Xiao, E., & Houser, D. (2005). Emotion expression in human punishment behaviour. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102(20), 7398–7401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Jack Knetsch, Daniel Houser and seminar participants at George Mason University for their helpful comments and suggestions, and Wu Jun for excellent research assistance. We also acknowledge financial support from Nanyang Technological University.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Steven M. Sheffrin.

Electronic Supplementary Material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Instructions for Proposers (DOCX 37 kB)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Ong, Q., Riyanto, Y.E. & Sheffrin, S.M. How does voice matter? Evidence from the ultimatum game. Exp Econ 15, 604–621 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-012-9316-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-012-9316-x

Keywords

JEL Classification

Navigation