Skip to main content
Log in

The Power of Examples: Illustrative Examples Enhance Conceptual Learning of Declarative Concepts

  • Replication
  • Published:
Educational Psychology Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Declarative concepts (i.e., key terms with short definitions of the abstract concepts denoted by those terms) are a common kind of information that students are expected to learn in many domains. A common pedagogical approach for supporting learning of declarative concepts involves presenting students with concrete examples that illustrate how the abstract concepts can be instantiated in real-world situations. However, minimal prior research has examined whether illustrative examples actually enhance declarative concept learning, and the available outcomes provide weak evidence at best. In the three experiments reported here, students studied definitions of declarative concepts followed either by illustrative examples of those concepts or by additional study of the definitions. On a subsequent classification test in which learners were presented with examples and were asked to identify which concept the example illustrated, performance was greater for students who had studied illustrative examples during learning than for students who only studied definitions (ds from 0.74 to 1.67). However, the effects of illustrative examples on declarative concept learning depended in part on the conditions under which those examples were presented. Although performance was similar when examples were presented after versus before concept definitions (Experiments 1a–1b), classification accuracy depended on the extent to which examples of different concepts were interleaved and whether definitions were presented along with the examples (Experiment 2).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. This characteristic is also what distinguishes declarative concepts from declarative facts, which are non-abstract statements with truth values (e.g., George Washington was the first president of the USA; the capital of Ohio is Columbus).

  2. Examples for each concept were presented in separate blocks to align the schedule of presentation trials in the example groups with the schedule of presentation trials in the definition-only group. In the definition-only group, presentation of the definition for each concept was spaced across blocks, because a wealth of prior research has shown that massed restudy often has minimal to no benefit for learning (for reviews, see Cepeda, Pashler, Vul, Wixted, and Rohrer 2006; Dunlosky et al. 2013). Thus, massed restudy would have provided a very weak comparison group.

  3. Lower-familiarity and higher-familiarity students in each experiment did not significantly differ in age, education, or vocabulary (all ps 0.18–0.98, except for a significant 4 % difference in vocabulary and a 0.7-year difference in education favoring the higher-familiarity subset in Experiment 1a), although they may have differed on other factors not measured here. Differences in concept familiarity were related to the time of semester in which participants completed the experiment in Experiment 1a and Experiment 2, with 77 and 62 % of lower-familiarity participants completing the experiment in the first half of the semester versus only 36 and 33 % of higher-familiarity participants in the first half of the semester. Both of these experiments involved samples drawn from the Kent State participant pool, the majority of which consists of students enrolled in General Psychology (in which the relevant content domain tends not to be covered until later in the semester). In Experiment 1b, 72 and 70 % of lower-familiarity and higher-familiarity participants completed the experiment in the first half of the semester, but the Washington University participant pool includes a much large proportion of advanced undergraduates who likely had completed coursework in which they may have previously encountered the experimental concepts.

References

  • Allen, S. W., & Brooks, L. R. (1991). Specializing the operation of an explicit rule. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 3–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, L. R., Norman, G. R., & Allen, S. W. (1991). Role of specific similarity in a medical diagnosis task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 278–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carmines, E. G., & Zeller, R. A. (1979). Reliability and validity assessment. Thousand Oaks: Sage.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Cepeda, N. J., Pashler, H., Vul, E., Wixted, J. T., & Rohrer, D. (2006). Distributed practice in verbal recall tasks: A review and quantitative synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 354–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cortina, J. M., & Nouri, H. (2000). Effect size for ANOVA designs. Thousand Oaks: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeCaro, M. S., & Rittle-Johnson, B. (2012). Exploring mathematics problems prepares children to learn from instruction. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113, 552–568.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Di Vesta, F. J., & Peverly, S. T. (1984). The effects of encoding variability, processing activity, and rule-examples sequence on the transfer of conceptual rules. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 108–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunlosky, J., & Metcalfe, J. (2009). Metacognition. Beverly Hills: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunlosky, J., Rawson, K. A., Marsh, E. J., Nathan, M. J., & Willingham, D. T. (2013). Improving students’ learning with effective learning techniques: Promising directions from cognitive and educational psychology. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 14, 4–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griffin, M. M. (1993). Do student-generated rational sets of examples facilitate concept acquisition? Journal of Experimental Education, 61, 104–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hambrick, D. Z. (2003). Why are some people more knowledgeable than others? A longitudinal study of knowledge acquisition. Memory & Cognition, 31, 902–917.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hambrick, D. Z., Meinz, E. J., Pink, J. E., Pettibone, J. C., & Oswald, F. L. (2010). Learning outside the laboratory: Ability and non-ability influences on acquiring political knowledge. Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 40–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, R. (1990). The effect of elaboration on the acquisition of conceptual problem-solving skills from prose. Journal of Experimental Education, 58, 5–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hestenes, D., Wells, M., & Swackhamer, G. (1992). Force concept inventory. The Physics Teacher, 30, 141–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby, L. L., Wahlheim, C. N., & Coane, J. H. (2010). Test-enhanced learning of natural concepts: Effects on recognition memory, classification, and metacognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 1441–1451.

    Google Scholar 

  • Judd, C. M., & McClelland, G. H. (1989). Data analysis: A model comparison approach. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kalyuga, S., Rikers, R., & Paas, F. (2012). Educational implications of expertise reversal effects in learning and performance of complex cognitive and sensorimotor skills. Educational Psychology Review, 24, 313–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaminski, J. A., Sloutsky, V. M., & Heckler, A. F. (2013). The cost of concreteness: The effect of nonessential information on analogical transfer. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 19, 14–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karpicke, J. D., & Blunt, J. R. (2011). Retrieval practice produces more learning than elaborative studying with concept mapping. Science, 331, 772–775.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klausmeier, H. J., & Feldman, K. V. (1975). Effects of a definition and a varying number of examples and nonexamples on concept attainment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 67, 174–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Libarkin, J. (2008). Concept inventories in higher education science. National Research Council, Promising Practices in Undergraduate STEM Education Workshop 2.

  • Medin, D. L., & Schaffer, M. M. (1978). Context theory of classification learning. Psychological Review, 85, 207–238. 31 pgs.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morris, C. D., Bransford, J. D., & Franks, J. J. (1977). Levels of processing versus transfer appropriate processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 519–533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, G. (2004). The big book of concepts. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Myers, D. G. (2010). Psychology: Ninth Edition. New York: Worth Publishers.

  • Needham, D. R., & Begg, I. M. (1991). Problem-oriented training promotes spontaneous analogical transfer: Memory-oriented training promotes memory for training. Memory & Cognition, 19, 543–557.

  • Nosofsky, R. M. (1999). Examplar-based accounts of relations between classification, recognition, and typicality. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14, 700–708.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paas, F. G. W. C., & Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (1994). Variability of worked examples and transfer of geometrical problem-solving skills: A cognitive-load approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 122–133.

  • Pashler, H., & Harris, C. R. (2012). Is the replicability crisis overblown? Three arguments examined. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 531–536.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, D. J., & Mulligan, N. W. (2013). The negative testing effect and multifactor account. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39, 1287–1293.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rawson, K. A., & Dunlosky, J. (2012). When is practice testing most effective for improving the durability and efficiency of student learning? Educational Psychology Review, 24, 419–435.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reder, L. M., & Anderson, J. R. (1982). Effects of spacing and embellishment on memory for the main points of a text. Memory & Cognition, 10, 97–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reed, S. K., & Bolstad, C. A. (1991). Use of examples and procedures in problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 753–766.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rittle-Johnson, B., & Star, J. R. (2011). The power of comparison in learning and instruction: Learning outcomes supported by different types of comparisons. In J. P. Mestre & B. H. Ross (Eds.), Psychology of learning and motivation: Cognition in education (Vol. 55, pp. 199–222). San Diego: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., Weldon, M. S., & Challis, B. H. (1989). Explaining dissociations between implicit and explicit measures of retention: A processing account. In H. L. Roediger III & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of memory and consciousness: Essays in honour of Endel Tulving (pp. 3–41). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rohrer, D. (2012). Interleaving helps students distinguish among similar concepts. Educational Psychology Review, 24, 355–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, E., & Mervis, C. B. (1975). Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 573–605.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L., Gilbert, D. T. & Wegner, D. M. (2009). Psychology. New York: Worth Publishers.

  • Schwartz, D. L., Chase, C. C., Oppezzo, M. A., & Chin, D. B. (2011). Practicing versus inventing with contrasting cases: The effects of telling first on learning and transfer. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103, 759–775.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spilich, G. J., Vesonder, G. T., Chiesi, H. L., & Voss, J. F. (1979). Text processing of domain-related information for individuals with high and low domain knowledge. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 565–583.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, K., & Rohrer, D. (2010). The effects of interleaved practice. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 837–848.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van den Broek, P. (2010). Using texts in science education: Cognitive processes and knowledge representation. Science, 328, 453–456.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zachary, R. (1986). Shipley Institute of living scale revised manual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimbardo, P. G., Johnson, R. L. & McCann, V. (2012). Psychology: Core Concepts (Seventh Edition). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson.

  • Zwaan, R. A., & Radvansky, G. A. (1998). Situation models in language comprehension and memory. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 162–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The research reported here was supported by a James S. McDonnell Foundation 21st Century Science Initiative in Bridging Brain, Mind and Behavior Collaborative Award.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Katherine A. Rawson.

Appendix

Appendix

Table 5 Performance on primary dependent variables for participants who indicated pre-experimental familiarity with four or more concepts

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Rawson, K.A., Thomas, R.C. & Jacoby, L.L. The Power of Examples: Illustrative Examples Enhance Conceptual Learning of Declarative Concepts. Educ Psychol Rev 27, 483–504 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-014-9273-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-014-9273-3

Keywords

Navigation