Skip to main content

A Model of Brain Activity of Young as Compared to Adult Dyslexic Readers and Outcomes After Intervention

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Reading, Writing, Mathematics and the Developing Brain: Listening to Many Voices

Part of the book series: Literacy Studies ((LITS,volume 6))

  • 2299 Accesses

Abstract

The chapter examines whether the cognitive deficits that young dyslexic’s (4th graders) exhibit are similar to those of adult university students dyslexics, in addition to their outcomes of intervention. This question was investigated with the use of behavioral, electrophysiological and source estimation of brain activation measures. The results revealed a different pattern of the behavioral profile and brain activity among the dyslexic in both age groups. This may indicate that compensatory mechanisms developed among adult dyslexics during their years of print exposure, and they manage to overcome some of their difficulties. In spite of their differences in the behavioral measures both age groups of dyslexic share similar characteristics, mainly in brain activity patterns. Both age groups benefited from the intervention although the different outcomes in each age group.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Bell, S. M., McCallum, S. R., & Cox, E. A. (2003). Towards a research-based assessment of dyslexia: Using cognitive measures to identify reading disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36, 505–516.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Dror, I., Pollatsek, A., & Scarpati, S. (1991). Word identification in isolation and in context by college dyslexic students. Brain and Language, 31, 308–327.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bentin, S. (1989). Electrophysiological studies of visual word perception, lexical organization, and semantic processing: A tutorial review. Language and Speech, 32, 205–220.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Breznitz, Z. (1997a). Enhancing the reading of dyslexics by reading acceleration and auditory masking. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 103–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Breznitz, Z. (1997b). The effect of accelerated reading rate on memory for text among dyslexic readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 287–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breznitz, Z. (2006). Reading fluency: Synchronization of processes. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breznitz, Z., & Berman, L. (2003). Reading rate as a dependent variable: A review. Educational Psychology Review, 15, 247–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Breznitz, Z., & Lebovitz, L. (2008). Neurobiological correlates of dyslexia. In Z. Breznitz (Ed.), Brain research in language (pp. 7–50). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Breznitz, Z., & Leiken, M. (2000). Effects of accelerated reading rate on processing words’ syntactic functions by normal and dyslexic readers: Event related potentials evidence. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 162, 276–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Breznitz, Z., & Nevat, M. (2004). Reading acceleration program. University of Haifa [Unpublished].

    Google Scholar 

  • Breznitz, Z., & Share, D. L. (1992). The effect of accelerated reading rate on memory for text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 193–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Breznitz, Z., DeMarco, T., & Hakerem, G. (1993). Topographic measures of cerebral activity during reading of text at fast-and-slow paced rates. Brain Topography, 6, 117–121.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Breznitz, Z., DeMarco, T., Shammi, P., & Hakerem, G. (1994). Self-paced versus fast-paced reading rates and their effect upon comprehension and event-related potentials. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 155, 397–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruck, M. (1990). Word-recognition skills of adults with childhood diagnoses of dyslexia. Developmental Psychology, 26, 439–454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruck, M. (1992). Persistence of dyslexics’ phonological awareness deficits. Developmental Psychology, 28, 874–886.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruck, M. (1998). Outcomes of adults with childhood histories of dyslexia. In C. Hulme & J. R. Malatesha (Eds.), Reading and spelling: Development and disorders (pp. 179–200). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brunswick, N., McCrory, E., Price, C. J., Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (1999). Explicit and implicit processing of words and pseudowords by adult developmental dyslexics. Brain, 122, 1901–1917.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Corcos, E., & Willows, D. M. (1993). The processing of orthographic information. In D. N. Willows, R. S. Kruk, & E. Corcos (Eds.), Visual processing in reading and reading disabilities (pp. 163–190). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins, D. L., Neelin, P., Peters, T. M., & Evans, A. C. (1994). Automatic 3D intersubject registration of MR volumetric data in standardized Talairach space. Journal of Computational Assist Tomography, 18, 192–205.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elbro, C., Nielsen, I., & Peterson, D. K. (1994). Dyslexia in adults: Evidence for deficits in non-word reading and in the phonological representation of lexical items. Annals of Dyslexia, 44, 205–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erez, A., & Pratt, H. (1992). Auditory event-related potentials among dyslexic and normal-reading children: 3CLT and midline comparisons. International Journal of Neuroscience, 63, 247–264.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Flowers, D. L. (1995). Neuropsychological profiles of persistent reading disability and reading improvement. In C. K. Leong & R. M. Joshi (Eds.), Developmental and acquired dyslexia (pp. 61–77). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, A. M., Laxon, V., Armstrong, E., & Frith, U. (1996). Phonological difficulties in high-functioning dyslexics. Reading and Writing, 8, 499–509.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gottardo, A., Siegal, L. S., & Stanovich, K. E. (1997). The assessment of adults with reading disabilities: What can we learn from experimental tasks? Journal of Research in Reading, 20, 42–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Isreal, J. B., Chesney, G. L., Wickens, C. D., & Donchin, E. (1980). P300 and tracking difficulty: Evidence for multiple resources in dual-task performance. Psychophysiology, 17, 259–273.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Karni, A., Meyer, G., Rey-Hipolito, C., Jezzard, P., Adams, M. M., Turner, R., & Ungereider, L. G. (1998). The acquisition of skilled motor performance: Fast and slow experience driven changes in primary motor cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science of the United States of America, 95, 381–868.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lefly, D. L., & Pennington, B. F. (1991). Spelling errors and reading fluency in compensated adult dyslexics. Annals of Dyslexia, 41, 143–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leong, C. K. (1999). Phonological and morphological processing in adult students with learning/reading disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 224–238.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levinthal, C. F., & Hornung, M. (1992). Orthographic and phonological coding during visual word matching as related to reading and spelling abilities in college students. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 4, 231–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lovegrove, W. (1993). Weakness in the transient visual system: A causal factor in dyslexia? Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 682, 57–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyon, G., Shaywitz, S., & Shaywitz, B. (1993). A definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 53, 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neville, H. J., Coffey, S. A., Holcomb, P. J., & Tallal, P. (1993). The neurobiology of sensory and language processing in language-impaired children. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 235–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (1994). Comparison of deficits in cognition and motor skills among children with dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 44, 147–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, B., Nasman, V. T., & Wilson, G. F. (1994). Task detection difficulty: Effects on ERPs in a same-different letter classification task. Biological Psychology, 38, 199–214.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Marqui, R. D., Esslen, M., Kochi, K., & Lehmann, D. (2002). Functional imaging with low resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (LORETA): Review, new comparisons, and new validation. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology, 30, 81–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Marqui, R. D., Michel, C. M., & Lehmann, D. (1994). Low resolution electromagnetic tomography: A new method for localizing electrical activity in the brain. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 18, 49–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ransby, M. J., & Swanson, L. H. (2003). Reading comprehension skills of young adults with childhood diagnoses of dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36, 538–555.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sarid, M. (1997). Orthographic processing and phonological processing test. Unpublished test. Haifa, Israel: Haifa Universit.

    Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (1994). Deficient phonological processing in disabled readers implicates processing deficits beyond the phonological module. In K. P. van den Bos, L. S. Siegel, D. J. Bakker, & D. L. Share (Eds.), Current directions in dyslexia research (pp. 149–167). Amsterdam: Swets & Zeitlinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shaul, S. (2005). The characteristics of young and adult dyslexic readers on reading and reading related cognitive tasks as compared to normal readers. Dyslexia, 11, 132–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaywitz, S. E. (1996). Dyslexia. Scientific American, 275, 98–140.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shaywitz, E. S., Shaywitz, B. A., Pugh, K. R., Fulbright, R. K., Constable, R. T., Mencl, W. E., et al. (1998). Functional disruption in organization of the brain for reading in dyslexia. Neurobiology, 95, 2636–2641.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shaywitz, S. E., Fletcher, J. M., Holahan, J. M., Shneider, A. E., Marchione, K. E., Stueberg, K. K., et al. (1999). Persistence of dyslexia: The Connecticut longitudinal study at adolescence. Pediatrics, 104, 1351–1359.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shaywitz, S. E., Morris, R., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2008). The education of dyslexic children from childhood to young adulthood. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 451–475.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L. S., Share, D., & Geva, E. (1995). Evidence for superior orthographic skills in dyslexics. American Psychological Society, 6, 250–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M. J. (1995). Phonological processing and developmental dyslexia. Journal of Research in Reading, 18, 132–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M. J., & Nation, K. A. (1997). Language, phonology, and learning to read. In C. Hulme & M. Snowling (Eds.), Dyslexia: Biology, cognition and intervention (pp. 153–166). London: Whurr.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M., Nation, K., Moxham, P., Gallagher, A., & Frith, U. (1997). Phonological processing skills of dyslexic students in higher education: A preliminary report. Journal of Research in Reading, 20, 31–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R. (2000). The foundations of literacy. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 89–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F. R., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J., & Scanlon, D. M. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What have we learned in the past four decades? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 2–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., & Barker, T. A. (1994). The development of orthographic processing ability. Neuropsychology and Cognition, 8, 243–276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, G. F., Swain, C. R., & Ullsperger, P. (1998). ERP components elicited in response to warning stimuli: The influence of task difficulty. Biological Psychology, 47, 137–158.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yap, R., & van der Leij, A. (1993). Word processing in dyslexics. Reading and Writing, 5, 261–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Shelley Shaul Ph.D. .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2012 Springer Science+Business Media B.V.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Shaul, S. (2012). A Model of Brain Activity of Young as Compared to Adult Dyslexic Readers and Outcomes After Intervention. In: Breznitz, Z., Rubinsten, O., Molfese, V., Molfese, D. (eds) Reading, Writing, Mathematics and the Developing Brain: Listening to Many Voices. Literacy Studies, vol 6. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-4086-0_5

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics