Skip to main content

Intentions and Communication: Cognitive Strategies, Metacognition and Social Cognition

  • Chapter
Neuropsychology of Communication

Abstract

Communicating involves different complex processes. A prerequisite of communication is the intentional planning of the entire process, that is, the conscious selection of meanings, adequate behavioral strategies, and communicative goals. In particular, we define an intentionalization process as a dynamic operation that actively involves speaker and listener: the former is engaged in the formulation and expression of his or her own intention to produce an effect on the listener (intentionalization); the listener, in turn, needs to interpret the speaker’s communicative intention (re-intentionalization) [13]. An intentionalization process consists of a number of functions that allow the communicative process to take place: the definition of communicative goals; the establishment of behavioral strategies for communication performance; the monitoring and self-monitoring of the communicative process; and the feedback produced by the interlocutor. The entire system makes use of a set of coordination competences that are essential to the regulation of intentions. In particular, the articulation of intentional systems requires the activity of attentional mechanisms aiming, on the one hand, to select information and to produce appropriate representations (representational communicative level), and, on the other hand, to organize the enactor system (communicative actions planning). Those operations implicate the activity of central coordination mechanisms, intended as the meta-level-controlled processes that require the speaker’s voluntary and conscious actions [4].In this chapter, we first focus on the relationship between intentionality and communicative intentions, highlighting the role of consciousness in communication.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 129.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Green GM (1996) Pragmatics and natural language understanding. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, New Jersey

    Google Scholar 

  2. Sperber D, Wilson D (1986) Relevance: communication and cognition. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  3. Balconi M (2008) Neuropragmatica [Neuropragmatics]. Aracne, Roma

    Google Scholar 

  4. Marraffa M, Meini C (2005) La mente sociale: le basi cognitive della comunicazione [The social mind: cognitive bases of communication]. Laterza, Roma

    Google Scholar 

  5. Csibra G (2004) Teleological and referential understanding of action in infancy. In: Frith CD, Wolpert DM (eds) The neuroscience of social interaction: decoding, imitating and influencing the actions of others. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 23–44

    Google Scholar 

  6. Tomasello M (1999) The cultural origins of human cognition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  7. Balconi M (2002) Neuropsicologia della comunicazione. In: Anolli L (ed) Psicologia della comunicazione. Il Mulino, Bologna

    Google Scholar 

  8. Frith U, Frith CD (2004) Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. In: Frith CD, Wolpert DM (eds) The neuroscience of social interaction: decoding, imitating and influencing the actions of others. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 45–75

    Google Scholar 

  9. Gallese V (2004) The manifold nature of interpersonal relations: the quest for a common mechanism. In: Frith CD, Wolpert DM (2004) The neuroscience of social interaction: decoding, imitating and influencing the actions of others. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 159–182

    Google Scholar 

  10. Dennett D (2001) Are we explaining consciousness yet? Cognition 79:221–237

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Zelazo PD (1999) Language, levels of consciousness, and the development of intentional action. In: Zelazo PD, Astington JW, Olson DR (eds) Developing theories of intention: social understanding and self control. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, New Jersey, pp 95–118

    Google Scholar 

  12. Balconi M (2006) Psicologia degli stati di coscienza: dalla coscienza percettiva alla consapevolezza di sé [Psychology of consciousness: from perceptual consciousness to self awareness]. LED, Milano

    Google Scholar 

  13. Kinsbourne M (1996) What qualifies a representation for a role in consciousness? In: Cohen JD, Schooler JW (eds) Scientific approaches to consciousness: the twenty-fifth annual Carnegie symposium on cognition. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, New Jersey

    Google Scholar 

  14. Spence DP, Smith GW (1977) Experimenter bias against subliminal perception? Comments on a replication. Brit J Psychol 68:279–280

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Kinsbourne M (1988) Integrated field theory of consciousness. In: Marcel AE, Bisiach E (eds) Consciousness in contemporary science. Clarendon Press, Oxford, pp 239–256

    Google Scholar 

  16. Gazzaniga MS (1998) The mind’s past. University of California Press, Berkeley

    Google Scholar 

  17. Baars BJ (2003) How does a serial, integrated, and very limited stream of consciousness emerge from a nervous system that is mostly unconscious, distributed, parallel, and of enormous capacity? In: Baars BJ, Banks WP, Newman JB (eds) Essential sources in the scientific study of consciousness. The MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, pp 1123–1130

    Google Scholar 

  18. Gazzaniga MS (1996) The cognitive neuroscience. The MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  19. Povinelli DJ, Bering JM (2002) The mentality of apes revisited. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 11:115–119

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Balconi M, Mazza G (2009) Consciousness and emotion: ERP modulation and attentive vs. pre-attentive elaboration of emotional facial expressions by backward masking. Mot Emot 33:113–124

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Balconi M, Crivelli D (2009) FRN and P300 ERP effect modulation in response to feedback sensitivity: the contribution of punishment-reward system (BIS/BAS) and behaviour identification of action. Neurosci Res 66:162–172

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Shallice T (1988) From neuropsychology to mental structure. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  23. Bottini G, Paulesu E, Sterzi R et al (1995) Modulation of conscious experience in peripheral sensory stimuli. Nature 376:778–780

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Feyereisen P (1988) Non verbal communication. In: Rose FC, Whurr R, Wyke MA (eds) Aphasia. Whurr, London, pp 46–81

    Google Scholar 

  25. Baddeley A (1996) Exploring the central executive. Q J Exp Psychol-A 49:5–28

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Gazzaniga MS (1997) Conversations in the cognitive neurosciences. The MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  27. Vallar G, Baddeley AD (1987) Phonological short-term store and sentence processing. Cogn Neuropsychol 4:417–438

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Vallar G, Baddeley AD (1984) Fractionation of working memory: neuropsychological evidence for a phonological short-term store. J Verbl Learn Verb Be 23:151–161

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Belleville S, Peretz I, Arguin H (1992) Contribution of articulatory reharsal to short-term memory: evidence from a selective disruption. Brain Lang 43:713–746

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Martin RC, Feher E (1990) The consequences of reduced memory span for the comprehension of semantic versus syntactic information. Brain Lang 38:1–20

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. McCarthy RA, Warrington EK (1990) Cognitive neuropsychology: a clinical introduction. Academic Press, San Diego

    Google Scholar 

  32. Waters GS, Caplan D, Hildebrandt N (1991) On the structure of verbal short-term memory and its functional role in sentence comprehension: evidence from neuropsychology. Cogn Neuropsychol 8:82–126

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Martin RC (1993) Short-term memory and sentence processing: evidence from neuropsychology. Mem Cognition 21:176–183

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Damasio AR (2000) The feeling of what happens: body and emotion in the making of consciousness. Vintage, London

    Google Scholar 

  35. Sommerhoff G (2000) Understanding consciousness. Its function and brain processes. Sage, London

    Google Scholar 

  36. Stuss DT, Alexander MP, Benson DF (1997) Frontal lobe functions. In: Trimble MR, Cummings JL (eds) Contemporary behavioral neurology. Butterworth-Heineman, Boston, pp 169–187

    Google Scholar 

  37. Frith CD, Wolpert DM (2004) The neuroscience of social interaction: decoding, imitating and influencing the actions of others. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  38. Baron-Cohen S (1995) Mindblindness: an essay on autism and theory of mind. The MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  39. Frith CD (2003) Neural hermeneutics: how brains interpret minds. Keynote lecture, 9th Annual Meeting of the Organization of Human Brain Mapping, New York

    Google Scholar 

  40. Mitchell JP, Mason MF, Macrae CN, Banaji MR (2006) Thinking about others: the neural substrates of social cognition. In: Cacioppo JT, Visser PS, Pickett CL (eds) Social neuroscience: people thinking other people. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 63–82

    Google Scholar 

  41. Morris JS, Frith CD, Perret DI et al (1996) A differential neural response in the human amygdale to fearful and happy facial expressions. Nature 383:812–815

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Philips ML, Young AW, Senior C et al (1997) A specific neural substrates for perceiving facial expressions of disgust. Nature 389:495–498

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Frith CD (2007) Making up the mind: how the brain creates our mental world. Blackwell Publishing, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  44. Caramazza A, Shelton JR (1998) Domain-specific knowledge systems in the brain: the animate-inanimate distinction. J Cognitive Neurosci 10:1–34

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  45. Adolphs R (2003) Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour. Nat Rev Neurosci 4:165–178

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Crivelli D, Balconi M (2009) Trends in social neuroscience: from biological motion to joint action. Neuropsychol Trends 6:71–93

    Google Scholar 

  47. Gallagher HL, Frith CD (2003) Functional imaging of “theory of mind”. Trends Cogn Sci 7:77–83

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Blackemore SJ, Decety J (2001) From the perception of action to the understanding of intention. Nat Neurosci 10:561–567

    Google Scholar 

  49. Sperber D, Wilson D (2003) Pragmatics, modularity and mind reading. Mind Lang 17:3–23

    Google Scholar 

  50. Kaplan JA, Brownell HH, Jacobs JR, Gardner H (1990) The effects of right hemisphere damage on the pragmatic interpretation of conversational remarks. Brain Lang 38:315–333

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Gardner H, Brownell HH, Wapner W, Michelow D (1983) Missing the point: the role of the right hemisphere in the processing of complex linguistic material. In: Perecman E (ed) Cognitive processing in the right hemisphere. Academic Press, New York, pp 169–182

    Google Scholar 

  52. Charolles M (1986) Grammaire de texte, théorie du discour, narrativité. Pratiques 11–12: 133–154

    Google Scholar 

  53. Schegloff EA (1972) Sequencing in conversational openings. In: Gumperz JJ, Hymes DH (eds) Directions in sociolinguistics. Holt, New York, pp 346–380

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2010 Springer-Verlag Italia

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Balconi, M. (2010). Intentions and Communication: Cognitive Strategies, Metacognition and Social Cognition. In: Balconi, M. (eds) Neuropsychology of Communication. Springer, Milano. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-88-470-1584-5_9

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-88-470-1584-5_9

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Milano

  • Print ISBN: 978-88-470-1583-8

  • Online ISBN: 978-88-470-1584-5

  • eBook Packages: MedicineMedicine (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics