Skip to main content

Mental Spaces

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Nature, Metaphor, Culture

Part of the book series: Cultural Linguistics ((CL))

  • 387 Accesses

Abstract

This chapter explores the emergence of mental spaces in which nature imagery may be rooted in, based on Fauconnier’s mental space theory (1985, 2007). The empiric analyses of folksongs provide evidence that the development of spaces, in which the representation of nature has an underlying role, follow specific cultural construal schemas. They are characterized by a movement from perceptuality to fictivity, and from external representation to internal expression, which is viewed here as a fulfillment of the cultural schema reservedness. Accordingly, the topics discussed are the reconstruction of mental spaces and the investigation of linguistic phenomena that support the schema. They include the consideration of Space Builders and Space Connectors, the identification problem of mental spaces, the transparency of transition from one space to another and also blended spaces. An important observation is that the entities of nature have a primary role in setting up various kinds of mental spaces while locating them in the physical space of the discourse. It is also highlighted that in the ambiguity and the specific evolvement of mental spaces in the folksongs, key notions are spatial grounding and the multiple temporal reference of Hungarian present tense verbs. Mental spaces are tied up with a range of construal operations, including epistemic grounding, perspectivity, and attention.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Brandt, L. (2009). Subjectivity in the act of representing: The case for subjective motion and change. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 8(4), 573–601.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cutrer, L. M. (1994). Time and tense in narratives and in everyday language. Doctoral dissertation. San Diego: University of California.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dinsmore, J. (1991). Partitioned representations. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Fauconnier, G. (1985). Mental spaces: Aspects of meaning construction in natural language. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fauconnier, G. (1997). Mappings in thought and language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Fauconnier, G. (2007). Mental spaces. In D. Geeraerts & H. Cuyckens (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive linguistics (pp. 351–376). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fauconnier, G., & Sweetser, E. (Eds.). (1996). Spaces, worlds, and grammar. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fauconnier, G., & Turner, M. (2002). The way we think: Conceptual blending and the mind’s hidden complexities. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johanson, L. (2000). Viewpoint operators in European languages. In Ö. Dahl (Ed.), Tense and aspect in the languages of Europe (pp. 27–188). Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kodály, Z., & Vargyas, L. (1952/1991). A magyar népzene. A példatárt szerkesztette Vargyas Lajos (12. kiadás) [The Hungarian folk music. The collection was edited by Lajos Vargyas]. Budapest: Editio Musica.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langacker, R. W. (1987). Foundations of cognitive grammar. Vol. I: Theoretical prerequisites. Stanford, California: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langacker, R. W. (1999). Grammar and conceptualization. Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Langacker, R. W. (2008). Cognitive Grammar: A Basic Introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liddell, S. K. (1996). Spatial representations in discourse: Comparing spoken and signed language. Lingua, 98, 145–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matsumoto, Y. (1996). Subjective motion and English and Japanese verbs. Cognitive Linguistics, 7(2), 183–226.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palffy-Muhoray, N. (2013). Future reference in Hungarian with and without future marking. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics (19)1, Article 17. http://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl/vol19/iss1/17. Accessed 16 March 2016.

  • Rosch, E. (1975). Cognitive reference points. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 532–547.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Szelid, V. (2010). Flowers of love. Acta Universitatis Sapientiae Philologica, 2(2), 221–236.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Hoek, K. (1997). Anaphora and Conceptual Structure. Chicago, London: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Judit Baranyiné Kóczy .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd.

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Baranyiné Kóczy, J. (2018). Mental Spaces. In: Nature, Metaphor, Culture. Cultural Linguistics. Springer, Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-10-5753-3_8

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-10-5753-3_8

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Singapore

  • Print ISBN: 978-981-10-5752-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-981-10-5753-3

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics