Skip to main content

Spatial Metaphors in Nature Imagery

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Nature, Metaphor, Culture

Part of the book series: Cultural Linguistics ((CL))

Abstract

In accordance with the proposal that the images of nature in folksongs unfold primarily in the dimension of space, this chapter sums up the principals of spatial representation that apply to the analysis of texts. The first part of the chapter discusses how perceptions regarding the environment affect spatial cognition and how it serves as a basis for cultural conceptualizations in terms of categorization and schematization. The issue of space and spatial cognition is approached from a linguistic perspective, based on the argument that while many concepts are widely recognized as pertaining to space (including location and placement, trajectory, landmark, direction, motion, etc.), other basic cognitive schemas should also be considered. A broader and more complex view is thus proposed, which includes, among others, construal operations such as perspectivisation, spatial grounding, reference point constructions, and subjective motion. Apart from acknowledging that spatial experience is per se universal, it is viewed as being grounded in culture partly due to language-specific features of spatial expressions, and due to the way in which metaphorical values are attached to spatial conditions, which are based on an agreement within a specific cultural group. It is argued that metaphorical meaning does not only emerge in the dimensional aspects of representation but also in the conceptualizer’s mental access to the referential scene.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    To talk about space and spatial relations […] languages make use of a relatively small number of elements […] I will refer to all these grammatical forms of language which express primarily spatial relations as spatial grams” (Svorou 1994: 31).

  2. 2.

    Cultural metaphors are also printed with small capitals, similarly to conceptual metaphors, however, the standard formulation is the use of as instead of is in metaphorical mappings (Sharifian 2017). This standard is consequently followed throughout the book in forming cultural metaphors.

  3. 3.

    The explanation of why a forest is ‘round’ in many folksongs is rather interesting. The adjective kerek ‘round’ originally referred to a specific type of forest: ‘small round-shaped forest’ or ‘a forest that fills the horizon’ (Bárczi 1951: 29–30). After losing its specific meaning, it became a common word for ‘forest,’ which was gradually replaced by another word, erdő ‘forest.’ The conventional compound in folk poetry, kerek erdő ‘round forest,’ is hence a tautological compound. Later, however, it changed its meaning to a specific shape of forest, transporting the semantic property ‘round’ and ‘full’ to forests, which had not been conceptualized as ‘round’ before (Horváth 1997: 300, see also Chap. 4).

References

  • Baranyiné Kóczy, J. (2014a). The metaphorical potential of space in Hungarian folksongs. Studia Linguistica Hungarica, 29, 51–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bárczi, G. (1951). A tihanyi apátság alapítólevele mint nyelvi emlék [The deed of foundation of the Abbey of Tihany as a linguistic record]. Nyelvészeti Tanulmányok 1. Budapest: Akadémiai Kiadó.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cuyckens, H. (1991). The semantics of spatial prepositions in Dutch: A cognitive linguistic exercise. Doctoral dissertation. Antwerpen: University of Antwerpen.

    Google Scholar 

  • D’Andrade, R. (1995). The development of cognitive anthropology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, G. W. (1980). Environmental cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 88(2), 259–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, V. (2004). The structure of time. Language, meaning and temporal cognition. Amsterdam, Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foley, W. A. (1997). Anthropological linguistics: An introduction. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horváth, K. (1997). Kereki, Csipkerek, Méhkerek és társai [‘Kereki,’ ‘Csipkerek,’ ‘Méhkerek’ and their likes]. A Magyar Nyelvtudományi Társaság Kiadványai, 209, 296–301.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. (1987). The body in the mind. The bodily basis of meaning, imagination and reason. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kövecses, Z. (2005). Metaphor in culture: Universality and variation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kreitzer, A. (1997). Multiple levels of schematiziation: A study in the Conceptualization of space. Cognitive Linguistics, 8, 291–325.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G. (1987). Image metaphors. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 2(3), 219–222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G. (1993). The contemporary theory of metaphor. In A. Ortony (Ed.), Metaphor and thought (2nd ed., pp. 202–251). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landau, B., & Jackendoff, R. (1993). ‘‘What’’ and ‘‘where’’ in spatial language and spatial cognition. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 16, 217–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Langacker, R. W. (1991). Foundations of cognitive grammar. Volume II: Descriptive application. Stanford, California: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langacker, R. W. (1993). Reference-point constructions. Cognitive Linguistics, 4(1), 1–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langacker, R. W. (1994). Culture, cognition and grammar. In M. Putz (Ed.), Language contact and language conflict (pp. 25–53). Amsterdam, Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Langacker, R. W. (1999). Grammar and conceptualization. Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Langacker, R. W. (2002). Deixis and subjectivity. In F. Brisard (Ed.), Grounding: The epistemic footing of deixis and reference (pp. 1–28). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lükő, G. (1942/2001). A magyar lélek formái [The shapes of the Hungarian soul]. Budapest: Táton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malcolm, I. G., & Sharifian, F. (2002). Aspects of aboriginal English oral discourse: An application of cultural schema theory. Discourse Studies, 4(2), 169–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, G. B. (1996). Toward a theory of cultural linguistics. Austin, Texas: University of Texas Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paradis, C. (1997). Degree modifiers of adjectives in spoken British English. Lund Studies in English (92). Lund: Lund University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paradis, C. (2001). Adjectives and boundedness. Cognitive Linguistics, 12(1), 47–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pütz, M., & Dirven, R. (Eds.). (1996). The construal of ppace in language and thought. Berlin, New York: Mouton DeGruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Regier, T. (1996). The human semantic potential: Spatial language and constrained connectionism. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rice, G. E. (1980). On cultural schemata. American Ethnologist, 7, 152–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, E. (1975). Cognitive reference points. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 532–547.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In E. Rosch & B. B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization (pp. 27–48). Hillsdale, New York: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sharifian, F. (2003). On cultural conceptualizations. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 3, (187–207).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sharifian, F. (2011). Cultural Conceptualizations and language: Theoretical framework and applications. Amsterdam, Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sharifian, F. (2017). Cultural Linguistics. Amsterdam, Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shore, B. (1996). Culture in mind: Cognition, culture, and the problem of meaning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sinha, C., & Bernárdez, E. (2015). Space, time and space-time: Metaphors, maps and fusions. In F. Sharifian (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of language and culture (pp. 309–324). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sinha, C., & Gärdenfors, P. (2014). Time, space, and events in language and cognition: a comparative view. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1326, 72–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sinha, C., & de Lopez, K. J. (2000). Language, culture and the embodiment of spatial cognition. Cognitive Linguistics, 11 (pp. 17–42).

    Google Scholar 

  • Strauss, C., & Quinn, N. (1987). Cultural models in language and thought. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Svorou, S. (1994). The grammar of space. Amsterdam, Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Szilágyi N., S. (1996). Hogyan teremtsünk világot? Rávezetés a nyelvi világ vizsgálatára [How to create a world? A guide to the study of the linguistic world]. Kolozsvár: Erdélyi Tankönyvtanács.

    Google Scholar 

  • Talmy, L. (1983). How language structures space. In H. L. Pick, Jr. & L. Acredolo. (Eds.), Spatial orientation: Theory, research, and application (pp. 225–282). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Talmy, L. (2000). Toward a cognitive semantics. Vol. 1. Concept structuring systems. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolcsvai Nagy, G. (1999). Térjelölés a magyar nyelvben [Space marking in the Hungarian language]. Magyar Nyelv, 95 (pp. 154–165).

    Google Scholar 

  • Tribushinina, E. (2008). Cognitive reference points: Semantics beyond the prototypes in adjectives of space and colour. Doctoral Thesis. LOT, Utrecht: Leiden University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, B. (1993). Cognitive maps, cognitive collages and spatial mental models. In A. U. Frank. & I. Campari (Eds.), Spatial information theory: Theoretical basis for GIS. Proceedings COSIT ’93. Lecture notes in computer science 716 (pp. 14–24). Berlin, New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, B. (2003). Structures of mental spaces: How people think about space. Environment and Behavior, 35(1), 66–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Hoek, K. (2007). Pronominal anaphora. In D. Geeraerts & H. Cuyckens (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive linguistics (pp. 890–915). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verhagen, A. (2007). Construal and perspectivisation. In D. Geeraerts & H. Cuyckens (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive linguistics (pp. 48–81). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whorf, B. L. (1956). Science and linguistics. In J. B. Carrol (Ed.), Language, thought, and reality: Selected writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf (pp. 212–214). Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zlatev, J. (2007). Spatial semantics. In D. Geeraerts & H. Cuyckens (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive linguistics (pp. 318–350). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Judit Baranyiné Kóczy .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd.

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Baranyiné Kóczy, J. (2018). Spatial Metaphors in Nature Imagery. In: Nature, Metaphor, Culture. Cultural Linguistics. Springer, Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-10-5753-3_3

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-10-5753-3_3

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Singapore

  • Print ISBN: 978-981-10-5752-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-981-10-5753-3

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics