Skip to main content

Part of the book series: Kluwer International Handbooks of Linguistics ((KIHL,volume 2))

  • 121 Accesses

Abstract

As explained in § 4.1, our analysis of the syllable structure of Imdlawn Tashlhiyt revolves around two theses, the Licit Consonantal Nuclei thesis and the Sonority-Driven Syllabification thesis. The evidence in favor of the Sonority-Driven Syllabification thesis was presented in the last two chapters. In this chapter we present our evidence in favor of the Licit Consonantal Nuclei thesis. According to this thesis, Imdlawn Tashlhiyt has no epenthetic vowels; if the nucleus in a syllable is not a full vowel it is a consonant.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Notes

  1. See also § 4.1.

    Google Scholar 

  2. For examples of that mapping, v. Dell (1984).

    Google Scholar 

  3. This assertion is valid only for those VTVs which occur between heterorganic consonants. Between homorganic consonants it is another matter, see below § 6.3.3.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Aspinion (1953: 120) likens the ‘e brefs’ of Aštuken Tashlhiyt to French schwa.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Mentioning the midsagittal region allows us to speak of the release of I, which we take to be a noncontinuant. On the relevance of pulmonic egressive airstream, v. Kim (1994).

    Google Scholar 

  6. This is in sharp contrast with the @ vowel of Rifian and Moroccan Arabic, which can occur between two voiceless consonants (v. § 6.5, § 8.2.2), or with fast speech pronunciations such as tkila for tequila in English, where a vowel can occur between t and k in slower speech (Hammond 1997: 34).

    Google Scholar 

  7. Homophonous with /ix t-lkm/ ‘if she reaches’ (if 3fs-reach).

    Google Scholar 

  8. Said, for instance, to a woman whose hair is hanging in front of her eyes.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Kinesthetic observations by ME suggest that sequences /fl, bl, ml/ may be counterexamples. We disregard these pending further research.

    Google Scholar 

  10. On ‘open’ and ‘close’ transitions, see Bloomfield (1933) and Catford (1977).

    Google Scholar 

  11. On articulatory overlap, see for instance Browman and Goldstein (1989, 1990).

    Google Scholar 

  12. Sonorant consonants are as a rule fully voiced in Imdlawn Tashlhiyt. They may devoice when they are subject to prepausal annexation after a voiceless consonant (v. DE 1985), but even in that context the devoicing is only a partial one.

    Google Scholar 

  13. This was already pointed out in DE (1985: 117).

    Google Scholar 

  14. Contextual factors other than the properties of the two consonants are also involved in determining the vowel quality of the intervening VTV. Coleman (2001) shows that the color of VTVs is influenced by neighboring vowels.

    Google Scholar 

  15. The expressions in (6) have the following meanings: (a) I covered for him; (b) I covered; (c) she lined it (clothing); (d) cause to vanish!; (e) he vanished; (f) wring the neck!; (g) wring (someone’s neck) for him!; (h) I wrung his neck; (i) she whipped; (j) she owns it; (k) you pulled; (1) you pulled for him; (m) you drowned; (n) he strangled; (o) she strangled you; (p) she bamboozled him; (q) you learned; (r) exchange!; (s) knead!

    Google Scholar 

  16. Note that /q/ is not included (see below).

    Google Scholar 

  17. The coronal closure in [istttut] is longer than that in the contrasting form [isttut] /is=t#t-ut/ ‘did she hit him?’ (int=do3ms 3fs-hit).

    Google Scholar 

  18. As already stated in § 3.1, we follow the proposals of Clements and Hume (1995) concerning the internal structure of segments.

    Google Scholar 

  19. There exists yet another variant, viz. dd.

    Google Scholar 

  20. But release is only optional in /mra t-ttu/ ‘if she had forgotten’.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Instances of sequences of more than two siblings were given in the text under (9).

    Google Scholar 

  22. In (12)a /d#t/ can also be pronounced as tt (but not as t2t, and in (12)b /k#kw/ can also be pronounced as a geminate kwkw (but not as kkw2kw). These free variants are due to regressive assimilations in phonation type and in rounding to which we will return later. These assimilations are optional in sequences of short consonants straddling a word boundary. When regressive rounding assimilation occurs in /k#kw in (12)b the preceding a is articulated further back. Only then does sentence (12)b become homophonous with the following: /is=akwkw ra-n tigmmi/ ‘the fact is that they even want the house’ (indeed=even want-3mp house).

    Google Scholar 

  23. That is, if they agree for the features [sonorant], [vocoid] and [approximant]. Clements and Hume’s [+vocoid] is the equivalent of [-consonantal] in Chomsky and Halle (1968), and t-vocoid] is the equivalent of [+consonantal]. Vocoids and liquids are [+approximant] whereas the other sounds are [-approximant].

    Google Scholar 

  24. The diagrams in this section represent only those aspects of the structure of segments which are relevant.

    Google Scholar 

  25. See Saa (1995).

    Google Scholar 

  26. See Dell and Tangi (1992: 158-159).

    Google Scholar 

  27. … unless there has been an assimilation in phonation type or in secondary labiality. Release is incompatible with assimilation between sibling stops, see DE (1996a: 385-388).

    Google Scholar 

  28. Contrary to the generalization stated at the beginning of this paragraph, there are a few contexts in which Fusion merges a geminate with a simplex sibling. For instance, in 2nd person imperfective forms, the prefix sequence /t-tt-/ must be realized simply as tt. Similarly, /dd-t/ must be realized as tt in /t-bidd-t/ ‘you stood up’ (tbitt). In all such cases, however, Fusion involves the loss of a skeletal slot, and its outc]ome abides by NO-TREBLE (see below).

    Google Scholar 

  29. u-king with=dir 3ms-come.

    Google Scholar 

  30. As formulated in (19), NO-TREBLE is too restrictive, for it excludes languages in which a nasal borrows its primary articulation from a geminate, for example /n+bb/ > mbb. A more adequate formulation is given in DE (1996a: 383).

    Google Scholar 

  31. We account for the optionality of release in sequences such as /tt#t/ by assuming that Fusion (13), which is an obligatory rule, is blocked by NO-TREBLE (19), and that SIBLING-RELEASE (11) is optional. A reviewer has pointed out an alternative: Fusion would be optional in sequences such as /tt#t/, and SIBLING-RELEASE would be obligatory in all contexts. We do not retain this alternative because it would force us to give up restriction (19), which enables us to link up the distribution of releases with the blockage of rules of complete assimilation (see Chapter 3) and with certain facts about the lexical distribution of adjacent identical consonants, on which v. below in § 6.4.1. Yet other evidence in support of (19) is provided by the behavior of the causative prefix before sibilant-initial kernels, v. DE (1996a: 381-385).

    Google Scholar 

  32. Impf 2-eat:impf-2mp.

    Google Scholar 

  33. On the others, see § 6.4.1.

    Google Scholar 

  34. The items in (21)a are all borrowings from Arabic. They all have a free variant with i after the second consonant, e.g. zmmim, xmmim, fnnin, etc]. The free variation between i and zero is also found in native verbs, viz. in the biconsonantal verbs where both consonants are obstruents, e.g. b(i)dd’ stand up’, bb(i)z ‘pound’, kk(i)s ‘remove’. In all such verbs one of the consonants is a geminate.

    Google Scholar 

  35. The uninterrupted triple m in zmmm sounds longer than the uninterrupted double m in tllmm ‘you (mp) spun’ (from /t-llm-m/).

    Google Scholar 

  36. In our 1985 article we stated that a simple consonant cannot immediately precede its geminate counterpart in a lexical entry, see (48)b p. 124. The existence of the items in (21)b shows that that assertion is false.

    Google Scholar 

  37. On the Obligatory Contour Principle see McCarthy (1986), Odden (1988) and references therein.

    Google Scholar 

  38. On syllabification and epenthesis in Ath Sidhar Rifian Berber, v. below in § 6.5.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Here are the glosses for the parenthesized forms: (a) id impf; (b) ‘hiding’ (deverbal noun, pluralia tantum); (c) ‘weave!’; (d-f) id p.

    Google Scholar 

  40. On the realization of causative /s-/, v. § 5.4.

    Google Scholar 

  41. In Tashlhiyt as in other Berber dialects, /dd/ is generally realized as tt in emphatic morphemes.

    Google Scholar 

  42. An exception must be made for vcd-vls sequences beginning with /b/, a consonant which is in some instances immune to devoicing in Imdlawn Tashlhiyt. In Imdlawn the extent to which regressive devoicing operates varies with the speakers’ age. It is more pervasive in the language of older speakers such as ME’s father. Even in ME’s less conservative dialect, the vcd-vls sequences in which regressive devoicing is only optional all belong to kernels which have transparent cognates in the local variety of Moroccan Arabic.

    Google Scholar 

  43. On vowel insertion in the imperfective, see § 5.2.

    Google Scholar 

  44. While the initial vowel belongs to the stem in Imdlawn, it is an augment in At Mangellat.

    Google Scholar 

  45. The Berber stratum also contains vestigial pairs which do not fit anymore into productive alternation patterns, e.g. a-bggas ‘belt’ / biks ‘gird!’ (see (24)f). The imperfective stem of biks is tt-bikis, not tt-bigis, which shows that in biks the velar consonant is lexically voiceless in present-day Imdlawn Tashlhiyt.

    Google Scholar 

  46. See e.g. Aspinion (1953).

    Google Scholar 

  47. On Haha Tashlhiyt, v. Ouakrim (1993) and Ridouane (1999). The latter discusses Haha spirantization in some detail.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Compare for instance Basset and Picard (1948) for Kabyle, Penchoen (1973) for Tamazight, and Aspinion (1953) and Destaing (1920) for Tashlhiyt.

    Google Scholar 

  49. For discussions of nearby dialects with rather similar phonological systems, see Chami (1979), Cadi (1981) and Chtatou (1982).

    Google Scholar 

  50. Oufae Tangi’s father and mother are from the Ath Sidhar area and Berber was her first language. She uses Berber with her parents and with other members of her family of their generation, some of whom are monolingual. She uses Arabic with her sisters and the people outside her family.

    Google Scholar 

  51. They are bare aorist stems. As in Tashlhiyt, such stems are used as 2s imperative forms.

    Google Scholar 

  52. RIGHT-TO-LEFT SCAN is almost identical with the epenthesis rule proposed in Saib (1976: 127) for the Ayt Ndhir variety of Tamazight. On that rule, see Hyman (1985: 68).

    Google Scholar 

  53. On how initial clusters fit in with this general scheme, see DT (1992).

    Google Scholar 

  54. See the references in note 49 for the works on Berber. For those on Moroccan Arabic, the references will be found in Chapter 8.

    Google Scholar 

  55. The angled brackets around the do3ms clitic indicate that it is extrametrical; in view of this it is disregarded by RIGHT-TO-LEFT SCAN, see below.

    Google Scholar 

  56. In Moroccan Arabic, according to Heath (1987: 184), schwa deletion in word-final syllables is blocked by ‘list intonation’, and in Japanese high vowel devoicing is blocked ‘when a final syllable in the devoicing environment must carry a rising intonation’ (Vance 1987: 51).

    Google Scholar 

  57. See Basset and Picard (1948: 9), Mitc]hell (1957: 197-198), Penchoen (1973: 10, 94). Extensive data elicited from Fouad Saa show that a situation similar to that just described also prevails in the Figuig dialect, on which see Kossmann (1994) and Saa (1995).

    Google Scholar 

  58. E.g. Harrell (1962a), Mitc]hell (1993: 62, 64), Shoul (1995: 208).

    Google Scholar 

  59. θ and θ do not have a geminate counterpart. Historically they derive from simplex t and d and still alternate with them, but they must be considered independent phonemes. One of the reasons for this state of affairs is a massive influx of Arabic loans with unspirantized t and d.

    Google Scholar 

  60. The corresponding masculine noun is aarraz /a-ʔrraz/. As a rule /rr/ surfaces as arr. /r/ is realized as r, ar or a depending on the context. See DT (1993) for a detailed discussion of how these alternations link up with syllabification.

    Google Scholar 

  61. Absorption of an unstable vowel by a neighbouring sonorant has been ascribed to other Berber dialects, see Mitc]hell (1957: 194) and Penchoen (1973: 10, 94), and to Moroccan Arabic, see Mitc]hell (1993: 63, 72) and Heath (1987: 249-253).

    Google Scholar 

  62. The glides of Ath Sidhar Rifian are discussed in detail in DT (1992).

    Google Scholar 

  63. V. also Levin’s (1987) distinction between epenthesis and excrescence. The facts of Ath Sidhar Rifian suggest that when epenthetic vowels and excrescent vocoids coexist in the same language, they need not have different vowel qualities.

    Google Scholar 

  64. As in Tashlhiyt, such nouns begin with a prefix /1-/, see § 2.5.3.1. The prefix assimilates to a following coronal; otherwise it surfaces as r, as do most occurrences of simplex /1/ in Ath Sidhar Rifian. On /r/ and /1/ in Ath Sidhar Rifian see DT (1993).

    Google Scholar 

  65. The causative prefix assimilates to a following sibilant, v. § 5.4.

    Google Scholar 

  66. On extrametricality, see e.g. Hayes (1995) and references therein.

    Google Scholar 

  67. Basing themselves on the limited data in Tangi (1991), DT (1992: 134) stated incorrectly that only coronal obstruents can be extrametrical. Subsequent work with Oufae Tangi has turned up nativized loans like s-s@rk ((27)c) and r-m@sk ((27)e), which end with non-coronals.

    Google Scholar 

  68. The initial /1-/ assimilates to the following coronal, hence /rr/, which is realized as arr.

    Google Scholar 

  69. Kossmann (1995: 80) presents similar facts concerning Ait Said Rifian.

    Google Scholar 

  70. Plural ssbayy. The MA source noun is !sbay-a (p !sbayy).

    Google Scholar 

  71. The fourth is extrametrical. It is the final /-θ/ which marks the feminine in /θ-…-m-<θ>/ ‘2fp’ and in /-n(<θ>/ ‘3fp’.

    Google Scholar 

  72. V. the i-epenthesis mentioned in note 35, as well as that presented in DE (1989: 191-193). Both are optional and morphologically-governed.

    Google Scholar 

  73. The facts which make this statement only a first approximation fall into three categories: (1) the special behaviour of prefix boundaries with respect to Fusion (v. § 6.3.3.3), (2) the erasure of one X slot in certain Pword-internal XXX sequences in which all three X slots are linked to sibling segments (for instance the prefix sequence /t-tt-/ must be realized as tt when /t-/ is one of the PNG prefixes and /tt-/ is the imperfective prefix, v. DE (1989: 193), and (3) the operation of Fusion in sequences of three or more simplex consonants which are siblings, v. the examples illustrating the three-way contrast between t2t:2t, t2t: and t2t2t: in the text below (9) in § 6.3.3.1.

    Google Scholar 

  74. See Clements (1997) for some discussion of multistratal syllabification in Imdlawn Tashlhiyt.

    Google Scholar 

  75. The authors of this book have since carried out a systematic survey of nominal and verbal kernels in Imdlawn Tashlhiyt and found that verbal kernels are subject to maximal size requirements akin to those which constrain nominal kernels.

    Google Scholar 

  76. See for instance Jebbour (1988) for number inflection in nouns (Tiznit Tashlhiyt), DE (1991) for verbal stem formation and DE (1992) for derivational morphology (Imdlawn Tashlhiyt).

    Google Scholar 

  77. The TIRRUGZA derivatives are pluralia tantum nouns in the feminine. See DE (1992) for a list of forms and a discussion of the templatic mapping.

    Google Scholar 

  78. The base nouns in (36)-I have the following meanings: (a) man, (b) free person, (c) guest, (d) dummy in a card game, (e) apprentice, (f) wealthy person.

    Google Scholar 

  79. Imperfective gemination is an exception to this generalization.

    Google Scholar 

  80. Basset (1952: 8).

    Google Scholar 

  81. Basset (1952: 5) and Galand (1953).

    Google Scholar 

  82. Some of the inconsistencies in these transcriptions probably come from variations in transcriptional practice, rather than from variation in the data. See Galand (1953: 230) for examples of inconsistencies in the transcriptions in Destaing (1920).

    Google Scholar 

  83. According to Basset and Picard (1948: 9) ʕ is ‘a vocalic element […] which ranges, depending in particular on tempo, from a well-marked vowel to nothing at all. All vowels are voiced; it may be the case, however, that ʕ could devoice, leaving a mere suspension as its only residue’. Penchoen (1973: 10) writes that what he transcribes uniformly as ʔ ‘may be — phonetically — an [ʕ], the syllabicity of a consonant such as a nasal, lateral or /r/ or even a simple consonant release voiced or not’.

    Google Scholar 

  84. See for instance the rule of schwa epenthesis in Guerssel (1977: 271). As we have seen in § 6.5.1, Rifian Berber follows essentially the same pattern.

    Google Scholar 

  85. Galand (1953: 230) even hints that some transcriptions of Berber may have been influenced by assumptions about syllabification in Arabic.

    Google Scholar 

  86. See also Galand (1988: 213): ‘… most often, the numerous occurrences of [ʕ] found in works by Berberists reflect habits which are alien to Tashlhiyt.’

    Google Scholar 

  87. See Galand (1957) on the school founded in Rabat by General Lyautey in 1912.

    Google Scholar 

  88. In our view the RIPIs are terminal representations of the phonological component.

    Google Scholar 

  89. The meanings of the examples in (40) are the following: (a) it went numb; (b) she even hoarded; (c) she even behaved as a miser; (d) what will happen of you?; (e) for the cockroaches; (f) you drowned; (g) you painted; (h) I locked; (i) he strangled; (j) he strangled him; (k) broken branch; (1) he wrung (someone’s neck) for him; (m) take care of his mother!.

    Google Scholar 

  90. On the deletion of /d/ at the end of the future preverb /rad/, see DE (1989: 188).

    Google Scholar 

  91. As a result of voice assimilation and Fusion, /dt/ surfaces as geminate /tt/, see § 6.3.3.2.

    Google Scholar 

  92. On the reasons why we now prefer to work with orthometric syllabification, v. § 4.1.

    Google Scholar 

  93. ME was unsure whether the form contained one syllable or two.

    Google Scholar 

  94. The IFDQ syllabification of form (41)d violates constraint NoPICOR, which disfavors rimes in which an obstruent nucleus is followed by a coda with the same degree of sonority, see § 4.9.1.

    Google Scholar 

  95. See DE (1996b: 232) for the discussion of a case in point.

    Google Scholar 

  96. Here ‘our account’ refers to our characterization of voiced transitional vocoids in DE (1996a), which is the same as that presented in the earlier sections of this chapter.

    Google Scholar 

  97. t-guni is the bound state form of t-a-guni. The affirmative form corresponding to t-gwni is t-gwna.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2002 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Dell, F., Elmedlaoui, M. (2002). Vowelless Syllables. In: Syllables in Tashlhiyt Berber and in Moroccan Arabic. Kluwer International Handbooks of Linguistics, vol 2. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-010-0279-0_6

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-010-0279-0_6

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Dordrecht

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4020-1077-4

  • Online ISBN: 978-94-010-0279-0

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics