Skip to main content

Effect of Early Bilingualism on Metalinguistic Development and Language Processing: Evidence from Chinese-Speaking Bilingual Children

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Reading Development and Difficulties in Monolingual and Bilingual Chinese Children

Part of the book series: Literacy Studies ((LITS,volume 8))

Abstract

Psychologists have long been interested in the impact of early bilingualism on children’s cognitive development. However, despite vigorous investigation into the topic, research on the effect of bilingualism on metalinguistic awareness and language processing has remained largely unexplored, either producing mixed findings or revealing advantages that cannot be ascribed solely to bilingualism per se. Many Chinese-speaking children around the world speak a second language, usually one that is typologically distant from Chinese, which affords an opportunity for isolating extraneous factors and identifying effect of bilingualism per se and provides a fertile ground for studying theories of early bilingualism in a broader cross-linguistic context. The purpose of this chapter is two-fold. First, it provides a critical review of the conceptual and methodological challenges in conducting research on the metalinguistic development and language processing of bilingual children. Second, it reports two empirical studies with Chinese-speaking bilingual children that were designed to investigate structural sensitivity theory Kuo & Anderson (Scientific Studies of Reading, 14, 365–385, 2010), an augmented theoretical framework to examine bilingual cognitive advantage in the verbal domain.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Aslin, R. N., Saffran, J. R., & Newport, E. L. (1998). Computation of conditional probability statistics by 8-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 9, 321–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Zeev, S. (1977). The influence of bilingualism on cognitive strategy and cognitive development. Child Development, 48, 1009–1018.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E. (1986a). Children’s concept of word. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 15, 13–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E. (1986b). Factors in the growth of linguistic awareness. Child Development, 57, 498–510.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E. (1988). Levels of bilingualism and levels of linguistic awareness. Developmental Psychology, 88, 560–567.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E. (1999). Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind. Child Development, 70(3), 636–644.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E. (2001). Bilingualism in development: Language, literacy and cognition. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E. (2009). Claiming evidence from non-evidence: A reply to Morton and Harper. Developmental Science, 12(4), 499–501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E., Luk, G., & Kwan, E. (2005). Bilingualism, biliteracy and learning to read: Interactions among languages and writing systems. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 43–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E., Luk, G., Peets, K. F., & Yang, S. (2010). Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(4), 525–531.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E., Majumder, S., & Martin, M. M. (2003). Developing phonological awareness: Is there a bilingual advantage? Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 27–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E., & Martin, M. M. (2004). Attention and inhibition in bilingual children: Evidence from the dimensional change card sort task. Developmental Science, 7, 325–339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E., McBride-Chang, C., & Luk, G. (2005). Bilingualism, language proficiency, and learning to read in two writing systems. Journal of Educational Research, 97, 580–590.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E., & Shapero, D. (2005). Ambiguous benefits: The effect of bilingualism on reversing ambiguous figures. Developmental Science, 8, 595–604.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bloomfield, L. (1933). Language. New York: Holt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bock, J. K., Dell, G. S., Chang, F., & Onishi, K. H. (2007). Persistent structural priming from language comprehension to language production. Cognition, 104, 437–458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bock, J. K., & Griffin, Z. M. (2000). The persistence of structural priming: Transient activation or implicit learning? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 177–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruck, M., & Genesee, F. (1995). Phonological awareness in young second language learners. Journal of Child Language, 22, 307–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, R., & Sais, E. (1995). Accelerated metalinguistic (phonological) awareness in bilingual children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13, 61–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chambers, K. E., Onishi, K. H., & Fisher, C. (2002). Generalizing phonotactic regularities from brief auditory experience. Paper presented at the Eighth Conference on Laboratory Phonology, New Haven, CT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chambers, K. E., Onishi, K. H., & Fisher, C. (2003). Infants learn phonotactic regularities from brief auditory experience. Cognition, 87, 69–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chang, F., Dell, G. S., Bock, J. K., & Griffin, Z. M. (2000). Structural priming as implicit learning: A comparison of models of sentence production. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research [Special Issue], 29, 217–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, X., Anderson, R. C., Li, W., Hao, M., Wu, X., & Shu, H. (2004). Phonological awareness of bilingual and monolingual Chinese children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 142–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chinese Proficiency Test. (2010). HSK Level 6. Retrieved March 29, 2010, from http://lingomi.com/blog/hsk-lists-2010/?utm_source=en.wikipedia.org&utm_medium=referral

  • Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, H., & Halle, M. (1968). The sound pattern of English. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cromdal, J. (1999). Childhood bilingualism and metalinguistic skills: Analysis and control in young Swedish-English bilinguals. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 1–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cummins, J. (1978). Bilingualism and the development of metalinguistic awareness. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 9(2), 131–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, D., Raschke, V. R., & Pervez, Z. (2010). Syntactic awareness in young monolingual and bilingual (Urdu–English) children. Cognitive Development, 25, 166–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duanmu, S. (2003). The phonology of standard Chinese. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, V. C. M. (2007). Miami and north Wales, so far and yet so near: A constructivist account of morphosyntactic development in bilingual children. The International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 10(3), 224–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, V. C. M., Sabastian, E., & Soto, P. (2002). The emergence of linguistic person in Spanish-speaking children. Language Learning, 52(4), 679–722.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gomez, R. L. (2002). Variability and detection of invariant structure. Psychological Science, 13, 431–436.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hakuta, K., & Diaz, R. M. (1985). The relationship between degree of bilingualism and cognitive ability: A critical discussion and some new longitudinal data. In K. E. Nelson (Ed.), Children’s language (pp. 319–344). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, Z. S. (1951). Methods in structural linguistics. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kovacs, A. M. (2009). Early bilingualism enhances mechanisms of false-belief reasoning. Developmental Science, 12, 48–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kovacs, A. M., & Mehler, J. (2009). Flexible learning of multiple speech structures in bilingual infants. Science, 325, 611–612.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuo, L.–. J. (2009). The role of natural features in the acquisition of phonotactic regularities. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 38, 129–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuo, L. J., & Anderson, R. C. (2006). Morphological awareness and learning to read: A cross-language perspective. Educational Psychologist, 41, 161–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuo, L. J., & Anderson, R. C. (2008). Conceptual and methodological issues in comparing metalinguistic awareness across languages. In K. Koda & A. Zehler (Eds.), Learning to read across languages (pp. 39–46). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuo, L. J., & Anderson, R. C. (2010). Beyond cross-language transfer: Reconceptualizing the impact of early bilingualism on phonological awareness. Scientific Studies of Reading, 14, 365–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuo, L.-J., & Anderson, R. C. (2012). Effects of early bilingualism on learning phonological regularities in a new language. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111, 455–467.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuo, L.-J., Uchikoshi, Y., Kim, T.-J., Li, Y., & Kowalczyk, E. (2011, April). Reconceptualizing bilingual effect on the development of phonological awareness: A study with children in general education and Japanese-English immersion programs, American Association of Applied Linguistics Annual Meeting, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lipka, O., & Siegel, L. S. (2007). The development of reading skills in children with English as a second language. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11(2), 105–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lipka, O., Siegel, L. S., & Vukovic, R. (2005). The literacy skills of English language learners in Canada. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 20(1), 39–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin-Rhee, M. M., & Bialystok, E. (2008). The development of two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11(1), 81–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Freidman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, R., & McLaughlin, B. (1986). Experts and novices: An information processing approach to the good language learner problem. Applied Psycholinguistics, 7, 41–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oller, D. K., & Eilers, R. (2002). Language and literacy in bilingual children. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.

    Google Scholar 

  • Onishi, K. H., Chambers, K. E., & Fisher, C. (2002). Learning phonotactic constraints from brief auditory experience. Cognition, 83(1), B13–B23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R. A. (1966). Predictability and number of pairings in Pavlovian fear conditioning. Psychonomic Science, 4, 383–384.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saffran, J. R. (2001a). The use of predictive dependencies in language learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 44, 493–515.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saffran, J. R. (2001b). Words in a sea of sounds: The output of infant statistical learning. Cognition, 81, 149–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saffran, J. R. (2002). Constraints on statistical language learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 47, 172–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yelland, G. W., Pollard, J., & Mercuri, A. (1993). The metalinguistic benefits of limited contact with a second language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 423–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgement

 This research was supported by a Summer Research and Artistry Grant and a College of Education Deans’ grant from Northern Illinois University awarded to Li-Jen Kuo.

We are grateful to Chih-Ping Lo, Elena Lyutykh and Fred Lu for their assistance with data collection. We also thank the students, teachers, and administrators who so graciously participated in or facilitated this research.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Li-Jen Kuo .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2014 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Kuo, LJ., Kim, TJ. (2014). Effect of Early Bilingualism on Metalinguistic Development and Language Processing: Evidence from Chinese-Speaking Bilingual Children. In: Chen, X., Wang, Q., Luo, Y. (eds) Reading Development and Difficulties in Monolingual and Bilingual Chinese Children. Literacy Studies, vol 8. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-7380-6_9

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics