Skip to main content

Bioacoustics Theories

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Soundscape Ecology

Abstract

Four major hypotheses are considered relevant in the perceiving and communicating processes common to all animal species: the morphological adaptation hypothesis (MAH), acoustic adaptation hypothesis (AAH), acoustic niche hypothesis (ANH), and species recognition hypothesis (SRH).

The morphological adaptation hypothesis (MAH) refers to the role of body size as a biological constraint of the vocalization organs and their acoustic performances, confirming an inverse relationship between acoustic frequencies and body size.

The acoustic adaptation hypothesis (AAH) states that the environment is an important cause of modification and alteration of the acoustic signals. Dominant frequencies and other long-distance calls are the result of an interaction between the animals and the environment to maximize the efficiency of the emitted sounds. Frequency and structure of the acoustic repertoire are plastic traits that can be modified according to the environmental constraint.

The acoustic niche hypothesis (ANH) states that every species has a unique acoustic space in which to structure the sonic species-specific signature to reduce interspecific competition and to optimize intraspecific communication mechanisms.

The species recognition hypothesis (SRH) supposes that species living in sympatry try to reduce the risk of utilizing similar sonic traits that could confound species in reproduction and create the risk of hybridizations. This set of hypotheses has epistemic relationships to form a meta-bioacoustic theory.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 189.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 249.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 249.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Blumenrath SH, Dabelsteen T (2004) Degradation of great tit (Parus major) song before and after foliation: implications for vocal communication in a deciduous forest. Behaviour 141:935–958

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blumstein DT, Turner AC (2005) Can the acoustic adaptation hypothesis predict the structure of Australian birdsong? Acta Ethol 15:35–44

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boeckle M, Preninger D, Hödl W (2009) Communication in noisy environments. I: Acoustic signals of Staurois latopalmatus Boulenger 1887. Herpetologica 65(2):154–165

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boncoraglio G, Saino N (2007) Habitat structure and the evolution of bird song: a meta-analysis of the evidence for the acoustic adaptation hypothesis. Funct Ecol 21:134–142

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Both C, Grant T (2012) Biological invasions and the acoustic niche: the effect of bullfrog calls on the acoustic signals of white-banded tree frog. Biol Lett 8:714–716

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brandley N, Burns M (2007) Effects of habitat and location on chipping sparrow song characteristics. http://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/57457

  • Brown TJ, Handford P (1996) Acoustical signal amplitude patterns: a computer simulation investigation of the acoustic adaptation hypothesis. The Condor 98:608–623

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown TJ, Handford P (2000) Sound design for vocalizations: quality in the woods, consistency in the fields. The Condor 102:81–92

    Google Scholar 

  • Brumm H (2006) Signaling through acoustic windows: nightingales avoid interspecific competition by short-term adjustment of song timing. J Comp Physiol A 192:1279–1285

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burns M (2007) Song divergence of chipping sparrow in mixed forest and open habitats: testing the Acoustic Adaptation Hypothesis. http://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/57458/1/Burns_Melissa_2007.pdf

  • Daniel JC, Blumstein DT (1998) A test of the acoustic adaptation hypothesis in four species of marmots. Anim Behav 56:1517–1528

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Derryberry EP (2009) Ecology shapes birdsong evolution: variation in morphology and habitat explains variation in white-crowned sparrow song. Am Nat 174:24–33

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dubois A, Martens J (1984) A case of possible vocal convergence between frogs and a bird in Himalayan torrents. J Ornithol 125:455–463

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ey E, Fischer J (2009) The “acoustic adaptation hypothesis:” a review of the evidence from birds, anurans and mammals. Bioacoustics 19:21–48

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feng AS, Narins PM, Xu C-H, Lin W-Y, Yu Z-L, Qiu Q, Xu Z-M (2006) Ultrasonic communication in frogs. Nature 440:333–336

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher NH (2004) A simple frequency-scaling rule for animal communication. J Acoust Soc Am 115:2334–2338

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher NH (2007) Animal bioacoustics. In: Rossing TD (ed) Springer handbook of acoustics. Springer, New York, pp 785–802

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Henry CS, Wells MM (2010) Acoustic niche partitioning in two cryptic sibling species of Chrysoperla green lacewing that must duet before mating. Anim Behav 80:991–1003

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutchinson GE (1959) Homage to Santa Rosalia, or why are there so many kinds of animals? Am Nat 93:145–159

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kingery H (1996) American dipper: Cinclus mexicanus. Academy of Natural Sciences, Philadelphia

    Google Scholar 

  • Krause B (1993) The niche hypothesis. Soundsc Newsl 6:6–10

    Google Scholar 

  • Krause B (2012) The great animal orchestra. Little, Brown, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Malavasi R, Farina A (2013) Neighbours’ talk: interspecific choruses among songbirds. Bioacoustics 22(1):33–48

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marten K, Marler P (1977) Sound transmission and its significance for animal vocalization. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2:271–290

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morton E (1975) Ecological sources of selection on avian sounds. Am Nat 109:17–34

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Naguib M (2003) Reverberation of rapid and slow trills: implications for signal adaptations to long-range communication. J Acoust Soc Am 113:1749–1756

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Naguib M, Mennill DJ (2010) The signal value of birdsong: empirical evidence suggests song overlapping is a signal. Anim Behav 80:e11–e15

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nemeth E, Dabelsteen T, Pedersen SB, Winkler H (2006) Rainforests as concert halls for birds: are reverberations improving sound transmission of long song elements? J Acoust Soc Am 119(1):620–626

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Patten MA, Rotemberry JT, Zuk M (2004) Habitat selection, acoustic adaptation, and the evolution of reproductive isolation. Evolution 58(10):2144–2155

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Planquè R, Slabbekoorn H (2008) Spectral overlap in songs and temporal avoidance in a Peruvian bird assemblage. Ethology 114:262–271

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Podos J (1997) A performance constraint on the evolution of trilled vocalizations in songbird family (Passerifomes: Emberizidae). Evolution 51:537–551

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz JJ (1993) Male calling behavior, female discrimination and acoustic interference in the neotropical treefrog Hyla microcephala under realistic acoustic conditions. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 32:401–414

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seddon N (2005) Ecological adaptation and species recognition drives vocal evolution in neotropical suboscine birds. Evolution 59(1):200–215

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sinsch U, Lumkemann K, Rosar K (2012) Acoustic niche partitioning in an anuran community inhabiting an Afromontane wetland (Butare, Rwanda). Afr Zool 47(1):60–73

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slabbekoorn H, den Boer-Visser A (2006) Cities change the songs of birds. Curr Biol 16:2326–2331

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Slabbekoorn H, Ellers J, Smith TB (2002) Birdsong and sound transmission: the benefits of reverberations. Condor 104:564–573

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slagsvold T, Wiebe KL (2007) Learning the ecological niche. Proc R Soc B 274:19–23

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sueur J (2002) Cicada acoustic communication: potential sound partitioning in a multispecies community from Mexico (Hemiptera: Cicadomorpha: Cicadidae). Biol J Linn Soc 75:379–394

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vasconcelos TS, Rossa-Feres DC (2008) Habitat heterogeneity and use of physical and acoustic space in anuran communities in Southeastern Brazil. Phyllomedusa 7(2):127–142

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallschlager D (1980) Correlation of song frequency and body weight in passerine birds. Experientia (Basel) 36:412

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zelick R, Narins PM (1985) Characterization of the advertisement call oscillator in the frog Elutherodactylus coqui. J Comp Physiol A 156:223–229

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler L, Arim M, Narins PM (2011) Linking amphibian call structure to the environment: the interplay between phenotypic flexibility and individual attributes. Behav Ecol 22(3):520–526

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2014 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Farina, A. (2014). Bioacoustics Theories. In: Soundscape Ecology. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-7374-5_3

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics