Skip to main content

Prosodic Adaptation in Language Learning

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Pragmatics and Prosody in English Language Teaching

Part of the book series: Educational Linguistics ((EDUL,volume 15))

Abstract

Prosodic phenomena such as intonation, rhythm and accentuation form the basis of understanding and acquiring language. They are used to support the exchange of information in discourse (e.g., to mark important information in an utterance), but also to regulate the relationship among the interlocutors, for example, to show affect or indicate group membership. In our paper, we summarize current experimental findings in the area of prosodic adaptation and draw a link to first language acquisition and second language learning. The findings of the cited studies show that prosodic adaptation can serve a number of communicative functions and that it can be seen both as the result of an automatic process supporting language processing, as well as a mechanism employed in social signaling. In the final part of the paper, we discuss the implications of the experimental results for methods designed to support prosody learning by L2 speakers.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Chen (2004), however, examined the paralinguistic interpretation of the codes in a series of perceptual experiments and found varying tendencies in judgments of speakers of different languages.

  2. 2.

    Overall, languages are expected to exhibit the tendency to use grammatical categories directly derived from the paralinguistic meaning. Even in those languages where the linguistic implementation of intonation categories goes against the Biological Codes (as seems to be the case, e.g., for rising statements in Northern Irish or falling questions in Hungarian), speakers will still exploit the original phonetic effect to convey their attitudes and emotions (Gussenhoven 2002).

  3. 3.

    This English variety is commonly referred to as Black South African English.

  4. 4.

    Cf. Nilsenová and Nolting (2010) who report a stronger adaptation in pitch span and f0 at the utterance boundaries (the final f0 in the prime and initial f0 in the participant’s response) by 4- to 6-year-old children compared to adult speakers.

References

  • Atterer, M., and D.R. Ladd. 2004. On the phonetics and phonology of “segmental anchoring” of F0: Evidence from German. Journal of Phonetics 32: 177–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Babel, M. 2009. Phonetic and social selectivity in speech accommodation. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of California, Berkeley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bavelas, J.B., A. Black, C.R. Lemery, S. Macinnis, and J. Mullet. 1986. Experimental methods for studying “elementary motor mimicry”. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 10: 102–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernieri, F.J. 1988. Coordinated movement and rapport in teacher-student interactions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 12: 120–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boersma, P., and D. Weenink. 2012. Praat: Doing phonetics by computer [Computer program]. Version 5.3.04. Retrieved 12 Jan 2012 from http://www.praat.org/.

  • Bolinger, D. 1978. Intonation across languages. In Universals of human language. Vol II: Phonology, ed. J. Greenberg, 471–524. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Branigan, H.P., M.J. Pickering, J. Pearson, and J.F. McLean. 2010. Linguistic alignment between people and computers. Journal of Pragmatics 42: 2355–2368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brent, M.R., and J.M. Siskind. 2001. The role of exposure to isolated words in early vocabulary development. Cognition 81: B33–B44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruce, G., and I. Thelander. 2001. A pitch accent journey in southern Sweden. Working Papers 49, 14–17, Department of Linguistics, Lund University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chartrand, T.L., and J.A. Bargh. 1999. The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 76: 893–910.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, A. 2004. Universal and language-specific perception of paralinguistic intonational meaning. PhD thesis, LOT dissertation series, 102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheng, C.M., and T.L. Chartrand. 2003. Self-monitoring without awareness: Using mimicry as a nonconscious affiliation strategy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 86: 1170–1179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collier, R. 1991. Multi-language intonation synthesis. Journal of Phonetics 19: 61–73.

    Google Scholar 

  • Costa, A., M.J. Pickering, and A. Sorace. 2008. Alignment in second language dialogue. Language & Cognitive Processes 23(4): 528–556.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coulston, R., S. Oviatt, and C. Darves. 2002. Amplitude convergence in children’s conversational speech with animated personas. In Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on Spoken Language Processing, Denver, 2689–2692.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cruttenden, A. 1993. The de-accenting and re-accenting of repeated lexical items. In Proceedings of the ESCA Workshop on Prosody, Lund, Sweden, Sept 1993.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cruttenden, A. 1997. Intonation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • den Os, E. 1988. Rhythm and tempo of Dutch and Italian: A contrastive study. PhD thesis, Utrecht University, Utrecht, the Netherlands.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dijksterhuis, A., and J.A. Bargh. 2001. The perception-behavior expressway: Automatic effects of social perception on social behavior. Advances in Experimental Psychology 33: 1–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fernald, A. 1989. Intonation and communicative intent in mothers’ speech to infants: Is the melody the message? Child Development 60: 1497–1510.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fernald, A. 1992. Human vocalizations to infants as biologically relevant signals: An evolutionary perspective. In Signal to syntax: Bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition, ed. J.H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, and J. Tooby, 365–388. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernald, A., T. Taeschner, J. Dunn, M. Papoušek, B. Boysson-Bardies, and I. Fukui. 1989. A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers’ and fathers’ speech to preverbal infants. Journal of Child Language 16: 477–501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frome Loeb, D., and G.D. Allen. 1993. Preschoolers’ imitation of intonation contours. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 36: 4–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gregory, S.W. 1994. Sounds of power and deference: Acoustic analysis of macro social constraints on micro interactions. Social Perspectives 37: 497–526.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gregory, S.W., and S. Webster. 1996. A nonverbal signal in voices of interview partners effectively predicts communication accommodation and social status perceptions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 70: 1231–1240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gregory, S.W., K. Dagan, and S. Webster. 1997. Evaluating the relation of vocal accommodation in conversation partners’ fundamental frequencies to perceptions of communication quality. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 21: 23–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gregory, S.W., T.J. Gallagher, and B.E. Green. 1999. Spectral analysis of candidates nonverbal communication predicts national debate outcomes. Paper presented to the American Sociological Association meetings, Chicago, IL.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gregory, S.W., Jr. and T.J. Gallagher. 2002. Spectral analysis of candidates’ nonverbal vocal communication: Predicting U.S. presidential election outcomes. Social Psychology Quarterly 65: 298–308.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grice, M., M. D’Imperio, M. Savino, and C. Avesani. 2005. Strategies for intonation labeling across varieties of Italian. In Prosodic typology: The phonology of intonation and phrasing, ed. S.-A. Jun, 362–389. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guitar, B., and L. Marchinkoski. 2001. Influence of mothers’ slower speech on their children’s speech rate. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 44: 853–861.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gussenhoven, C. 2002. Intonation and interpretation: Phonetics and phonology. In Proceedings of speech and prosody’02, Aix-en-Provence, ed. B. Bel and I. Marlien.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gussenhoven, C. 2004. The phonology of tone and intonation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Haas, A., and S.W. Gregory. 2005. The impact of physical attractiveness on women’s social status and interactional power. Sociological Forum 20: 449–471.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halliday, M.A.K. 1967. Notes on transitivity and theme in English. Part 2. Journal of Linguistics 3: 199–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hart, J.T., R. Collier, and A.A. Cohen. 1990. A perceptual study of intonation: An experimental-phonetic approach to speech melody. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Irino, T., and R.D. Patterson. 2002. Segregating information about the size and shape of the vocal tract using a time-domain auditory model: The stabilized wavelet-mellin transform. Speech Communication 36: 181–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Juslin, P.N., and P. Laukka. 2003. Communication of emotions in vocal expression and music performance: Different channels, same code? Psychological Bulletin 129(5): 770–814.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kunzel, H. 1989. How well does average fundamental frequency correlate with speaker height and weight? Phonetica 46: 117–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ladd, D.R. 1996. Intonational phonology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • LaFrance, M. 1979. Nonverbal synchrony and rapport: Analysis by the cross-lag panel technique. Psychology Quarterly 42: 66–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LaFrance, M. 1982. Posture mirroring and rapport. In Interaction rhythms: Periodicity in communicative behavior, ed. M. Davis, 279–298. New York: Human Sciences Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • LaFrance, M., and M. Broadbent. 1976. Group rapport: Posture sharing as a nonverbal indicator. Group and Organization Studies 1: 328–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lakin, J.L., and T.L. Chartrand. 2003. Using nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create affiliation and rapport. Psychological Science 14: 334–339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lakin, J.L., V.E. Jefferis, C.M. Cheng, and T.L. Chartrand. 2003. The chameleon effect as social glue: Evidence for the evolutionary significance of nonconscious mimicry. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 27: 145–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leon, P.R., and P. Martin. 1972. Applied linguistics and the teaching of intonation. The Modern Language Journal 56: 139–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levis, J., and L. Pickering. 2004. Teaching intonation in discourse using speech visualization technology. System 32: 505–524.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, P. 1984. The biology and evolution of language. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, M., and J. Pierrehumbert. 1984. Intonational invariance under changes in pitch range and length. In Langauge sound structure, ed. M. Aronoff and R. Oehrle, 157–233. MIT Press, Cambridge MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maurer, R.E., and J.H. Tindall. 1983. Effect of postural congruence on client’s perception of counselor empathy. Journal of Counseling Psychology 30: 158–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McRoberts, G.W., and C.T. Best. 1997. Accommodation in mean f0 during mother-infant and father-infant vocal interactions: A longitudinal case study. Journal of Child Language 24: 719–736.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A.N., and M.K. Moore. 1977. Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science 198: 75–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mertens, P. 2004. The Prosogram: Semi-automatic transcription of prosody based on a tonal perception model, In Proceedings of speech prosody 2004, Nara, Japan, 23–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moltholt, G. 1988. Computer-assisted instruction in pronunciation for Chinese speakers of American English. TESOL Quarterly 22: 91–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nilsenová, M., and P. Nolting. 2010. Linguistic adaptation in semi-natural dialogues: Age comparison. In Text, speech and dialogue, ed. P. Sojka, A. Horák, I. Kopeček, and K. Pala. Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nilsenová, M., M. Swerts, V. Houtepen, and H. Dittrich. (2009) Pitch adaptation in different age groups: Boundary tones versus global pitch. In Proceedings of Interspeech 2009: 10th Annual Conference of the International Speech Communication Association, ed. M. Uther, R. Moore, and S. Cox, 1015–1018. Brighton: ISCA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ohala, J. 1983. Cross-language use of pitch: An ethological view. Phonetica 40: 1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ohala, J. 1984. An ethological perspective on common cross-language utilization of F0 in voice. Phonetica 41: 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papoušek, M., H. Papoušek, and B.J. Harris. 1987. The emergence of play in parent-infant interactions. In Curiosity, imagination, and play: On the development of spontaneous cognitive and motivational processes, ed. D. Görlitz and J.F. Wohlwill. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Papoušek, M., and H. Papoušek. 1989. Forms and functions of vocal matching in interactions between mothers and their precanonical infants. First Language 9: 137–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pardo, J. 2006. On phonetic convergence during conversational interaction. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 119(4): 2382–2393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parrill, F., and I. Kimbara. 2006. Seeing and hearing double: The influence of mimicry in speech and gesture on observers. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 30: 157–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pickering, M., and S. Garrod. 2004. Towards a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27: 169–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rietveld, A., and C. Gussenhoven. 1987. Perceived speech rate and intonation. Journal of Phonetics 15: 273–285.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryalls, B.O., and D.B. Pisoni. 1997. The effect of talker variability on word recognition in preschool children. Developmental Psychology 33: 441–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K. 1989. Vocal correlates of emotional arousal and affective disturbance. In Handbook of social psychophysiology, ed. H. Wagner and A. Manstead, 165–197. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, G.M., M. Cooper, J.L. Morgan, and R. Brenneise-Sarshad. 1990. Imitation of intonation by infants. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 33: 9–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. 1995. Issues in the study of input: Fine-tuning, universality, individual and developmental differences and necessary causes. In The handbook of child language, ed. P. Fletcher and B. MacWhinney, 180–194. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sluijter, A.M.C., and J.M.B. Terken. 1993. Beyond sentence prosody: paragraph intonation in Dutch. Phonetica 50: 180–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Soderstrom, M., M. Blossom, R. Foygel, and J.L. Morgan. 2008. Acoustical cues and grammatical units in speech to two preverbal infants. Journal of Child Language 35: 869–902.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spaai, G.W.G., and D.J. Hermes. 1993. A visual display for the teaching of intonation. CALICO Journal 10: 19–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Staum Casasanto, L., K. Jasmin, and D. Casasanto. 2010. Virtually accommodating: Speech rate accommodation to a virtual interlocutor. In Proceedings of the 32nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, ed. S. Ohlsson and R. Catrambone, 127–132. Austin: Cognitive Science Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swerts, M. 2007. Contrast and accent in Dutch and Romanian. Journal of Phonetics 35: 380–397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swerts, M., and S. Zerbian. 2010. Intonational differences between L1 and L2 English in South Africa. Phonetica 67: 127–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swerts, M., E. Krahmer, and C. Avesani. 2002. Prosodic marking of information status in Dutch and Italian: A comparative analysis. Journal of Phonetics 30: 629–654.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Termine, N.T., and C.E. Izard. 1988. Infants’ response to their mothers’ expressions of joy and sadness. Developmental Psychology 24: 223–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vallduví, E. 1992. The information component. New York: Garland Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verhoeven, J.De, G. Pauw, and H. Kloots. 2004. Speech rate in a pluricentric language: A comparison between Dutch in Belgium and the Netherlands. Language and Speech 47: 297–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • von Kriegstein, K., J.D. Warren, D.T. Ives, R.D. Patterson, and T.D. Griffiths. 2006. Processing the acoustic effect of size in speech sounds. NeuroImage 32: 368–375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ward, A., and D. Litman. 2007. Dialog convergence and learning. In Proceedings of the 13th International Conference on Artificial Intelligence in Education: Building Technology Rich Learning Contexts that Work, Los Angeles, CA, 262–269.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wichmann, A. 2010. Intonational meaning in institutional settings: The role of syntagmatic relations. Cultural Studies of Science Education 5: 849–857.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Willems, N.J., R. Collier, and J. ‘t Hart. 1988. A synthesis scheme for British English intonation. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 84: 1250–1261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Marie Nilsenová .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2012 Springer Science+Business Media B.V.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Nilsenová, M., Swerts, M. (2012). Prosodic Adaptation in Language Learning. In: Romero-Trillo, J. (eds) Pragmatics and Prosody in English Language Teaching. Educational Linguistics, vol 15. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-3883-6_6

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics