Skip to main content

Participating in Classroom Mathematical Practices

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
A Journey in Mathematics Education Research

Part of the book series: Mathematics Education Library ((MELI,volume 48))

Abstract

In this article, we describe a methodology for analyzing the collective learning of the classroom community in terms of the evolution of classroom mathematical practices. To develop the rationale for this approach, we first ground the discussion in our work as mathematics educators who conduct classroom-based design research. We then present a sample analysis taken from a 1st-grade classroom teaching experiment that focused on linear measurement to illustrate how we coordinate a social perspective on communal practices with a psychological perspective on individual students’ diverse ways of reasoning as they participate in those practices. In the concluding sections of the article, we frame the sample analysis as a paradigm case in which to clarify aspects of the methodology and consider its usefulness for design research.

The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 10(1&2) (2001), 113–163.

Copyright © 2001, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 139.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    A discussion of the purposes for conducting a classroom teaching experiment and of the theoretical and pragmatic issues involved can be found in Cobb (2000b), Confrey and Lachance (2000), Simon (2000), and Yackel (1995).

  2. 2.

    Following Gravemeijer (1994a), we have previously called this type of research developmental research. Our purpose in relabeling it design research is to reduce the possibility that it may be confused with either child development research or with noninterventionist research into the development of particular mathematical concepts.

  3. 3.

    For a justification of the notion of a classroom microculture, see Cole (1995).

  4. 4.

    We speak of normative activities being taken as shared rather than shared to leave room for the diversity in individual students’ ways of participating in these activities. The assertion that a particular activity is taken as shared makes no deterministic claims about the reasoning of the participating students, least of all that their reasoning is identical.

  5. 5.

    For a number of years, we spoke of the relation between the two perspectives as being dialectical. However, German colleagues pointed out that, in their country, this term was associated with neo-Marxist theories of dialectical materialism. It was to avoid this miscommunication that we began to describe the relation as reflexive.

  6. 6.

    Given our discussion of social and psychological perspectives, we need to say explicitly that we are not attempting to reconcile Vygotskian and Piagetian theory as some have assumed (e.g., Lerman, 1996).

  7. 7.

    In reflecting on his activity as an instructional designer, Gravemeijer (1994b) argued that design resembles the thinking process that Lawler (1985) characterized by the French word bricolage, a metaphor taken from Claude Levi–Strauss. A bricoleur is a handy man who invents pragmatic solutions in practical situations. The bricoleur has become adapt at using whatever is available. The bricoleur’s tools and materials are very heterogeneous: Some remain from earlier jobs; others have been collected with a certain project in mind. Extending this metaphor, we would add the interpretative framework we use can also be viewed as a bricolage. In developing it, we have acted as bricoleurs who have drawn on and adapted ideas from a range of theoretical sources for pragmatic ends. Casting our work in these down-to-earth terms serves to differentiate it from more grandiose efforts that aim to fashion theoretical cosmologies (cf. Shotter, 1995).

  8. 8.

    The characterization of the individual is generally consistent with Roth’s use of the individual’s life world or the individual in the world as his unit of analysis (Roth & McGinn, 1998). In his terms, our concern when we adopt the psychological perspective of the interpretive framework is to delineate the ontology of the world in which an individual student acts.

  9. 9.

    This approach of coordinating psychological and social analyses is closely related to several other proposals. These include Hatano’s (1993) call to synthesize constructivist and Vygotskian perspectives, Saxe’s (1991) discussion of the intertwining of cultural forms and cognitive functions, and Rogoff’s (1995) distinction between three planes of analysis that correspond to personal, interpersonal, and community processes.

  10. 10.

    In psychological terms, this learning can be described as the internalization and interiorization of the activity of creating two coordinated logs. This activity was socially situated in that we were participating in the practices of our research community as we conducted the analyses.

  11. 11.

    These interventions clearly changed the social interactions in which the target students engaged. We justify the interventions on the pragmatic grounds of generating information that we needed to make pedagogical decisions.

  12. 12.

    The emphasis that we give to quantitative reasoning can be contrasted with the almost exclusive emphasis on numerical reasoning both in traditional classrooms and in many classrooms where instruction is compatible with recent reform recommendations (cf. Smith, 1997; Thompson, 1993, 1994; Thompson & Thompson, 1996).

  13. 13.

    This is not to say that all explanations involved a backing. It was not necessary for a student to give a backing when other students indicated that they understood a particular method of measuring structured space.

  14. 14.

    Nancy’s gesturing as she explained, “Twenty is how many little cubes we’ve done so far” illustrates Roth’s (2001, this issue) arguments concerning the integration of word and gesture in establishing the entities that are spoken about. Several other examples of this phenomenon are apparent in our account of the emergence of the first three mathematical practices. In this connection, it is worth noting that we attended to the teacher’s gesturing as part of our instructional design. In particular, we conjectured that it would be important for the teacher to indicate by gesture the entire distance that had been measured when iterating a Smurf bar rather than the successive placements of the bar.

  15. 15.

    Bowers and Nickerson (1998) reported a similar pattern in their analysis of a university mathematics course that focused on quantitative reasoning.

  16. 16.

    See Thompson et al. (1994) for a more extensive discussion of the role of inscriptions in supporting conceptual conversations.

  17. 17.

    The important role of interactions in which an adult attributes greater competence to a child than the child is displaying features prominently in the Vygotskian literature (e.g., Newman, Griffin, & Cole, 1989; Stone, 1993).

  18. 18.

    This semiotic ecology can be made explicit by delineating the chain of signification (Lacan, 1977; Walkerdine, 1988) that is constituted as successive classroom mathematical practice emerge. Stephan (1998) described the chain of signification that was constituted during the measurement teaching experiment. Examples of other analyses of this type can be found in Cobb et al. (1997) and Gravemeijer et al. 2000).

  19. 19.

    Wertsch (1991) made a similar point when he observed that much contemporary research in psychology does not in fact have the practical implications claimed for it: “In my view, a major reason is the tendency of psychological research, especially in the United States, to examine human mental functioning as if it exists in a cultural, institutional, and historical vacuum” (p. 2).

  20. 20.

    It could be argued that the forms of instruction developed in the course of a teaching experiment are unfeasible for any teacher working alone. We would acknowledge, for example, that the entire research team in effect constitutes a collective teacher with some members of the team actually teaching, whereas others observe and analyze classroom events. The demands of this collective activity are, however, balanced by the possibility that the collaborating teachers will be able to capitalize on our learning as represented by instructional sequences and learning trajectories. This conjecture about the proposed role of instructional sequences as a means of supporting the development of professional teaching communities is discussed in some detail by Cobb and McClain (2001). As part of our work with teachers, we are currently developing a series of companion CD-ROMs to support their learning that are based on the video recordings and other data sources. Readers who are interested in the practical implications of our work are referred to a series of articles and book chapters that we have developed for practitioner audiences (e.g., McClain & Cobb, 1999; McClain, Cobb, & Gravemeijer, 1999; McClain, Cobb, Gravemeijer, & Estes, 2000).

References

  • Apple, M. (1995). Taking power seriously. In W. Secada, E. Fennema, & L. Adajian (Eds.), New directions for equity in mathematics education (pp. 329–348). NewYork: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, P., Delamont, S., & Hammersley, M. (1988). Qualitative research traditions: A British response to Jacob. Review of Educational Research, 58, 231–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Axel, E. (1992). One developmental line in European Activity Theory. Quarterly Newsletter of the Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition, 14, 8–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ball, D. (1993). With an eye on the mathematical horizon: Dilemmas of teaching elementary school mathematics. Elementary School Journal, 93, 373–397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ball, D. L., & Cohen, D. K. (1996). Reform by the book: What is—or might be—the role of curriculum materials in teacher learning and instructional reform. Educational Researcher, 25(9), 6–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barab, S. A., & Kirshner, D. (2001). Guest editors’ introduction: Rethinking methodology in the learning sciences. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 10(1&2), 5–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bateson, G. (1973). Steps to an ecology of mind. London: Paladin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bauersfeld, H., Krummheuer, G., & Voigt, J. (1988). Interactional theory of learning and teaching mathematics and related microethnographical studies. In H.-G. Steiner & A. Vermandel (Eds.), Foundations and methodology of the discipline of mathematics education (pp. 174–188). Antwerp, Belgium: Proceedings of the TME Conference.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, A. (1985). The social construction of meaning—A significant development for mathematics education? For the Learning of Mathematics, 5, 24–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blumer, H. (1969). Symbolic interactionism: Perspectives and method. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boaler, J. (2000). Exploring situated insights into research and learning. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 31(1), 113–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, J., Cobb, P., & McClain, K. (1999). The evolution of mathematical practices: A case study. Cognition and Instruction. 17(1), 25–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, J., & Nickerson, S. (1998, April 13–17). Documenting the development of a collective conceptual orientation in a college-level mathematics course. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Diego.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, T. P., & Franke, M. L. (1998). Teachers as learners. Principled Practice, 2, 1–3.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cobb, P. (1998a). Learning from distributed theories of intelligence. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 5, 187–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cobb, P. (1998b). Theorizing about mathematical conversations and learning from practice. For the Learning of Mathematics, 18, 46–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cobb, P. (1999). Individual and collective mathematical development: The case of statistical data analysis. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 1, 5–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cobb, P. (2000a). Accounting for mathematical development in the social context of the classroom. In L. P. Steffe (Ed.), Radical constructivism in action: Beyond the pioneering work of Ernst von Glasersfeld. London: Falmer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cobb, P. (2000b). Conducting classroom teaching experiments in collaboration with teachers. In A. E. Kelly & R. Lesh (Eds.), Handbook of research design in mathematics and science education (pp. 307–334). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cobb, P., & Bowers, J. (1999). Cognitive and situated perspectives in theory and practice. Educational Researcher, 28, 4–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cobb, P., Gravemeijer, K., Yackel, E., McClain, K., & Whitenack, J. (1997). Mathematizing and symbolizing: The emergence of chains of signification in one first-grade classroom. In D. Kirshner & J. A. Whitson (Eds.), Situated cognition theory: Social semiotic, and psychological perspectives (pp. 151–233). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cobb, P., Jaworski, B., & Presmeg, N. (1996). Emergent and sociocultural views of mathematical activity. In P. Nesher, L. P. Steffe, P. Cobb, G. Goldin, & B. Greer (Eds.), Theories of mathematical learning (pp. 3–20). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cobb, P., & McClain, K. (2001). An approach for supporting teachers’ learning in social context. In F.-L. Lin & T. Cooney (Eds.), Making sense of mathematics teacher education (pp. 207–232). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cobb, P., & Whitenack, J. W. (1996). A method for conducting longitudinal analyses of classroom video recordings and transcripts. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 30, 458–477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cobb, P., Wood, T., & Yackel, E. (1992). Learning and interaction in classroom situations. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 23, 99–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cobb, P., & Yackel, E. (1996). Constructivist, emergent, and sociocultural perspectives in the context of developmental research. Educational Psychologist, 31, 175–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cobb, P., Yackel, E., & Wood, T. (1989). Young children’s emotional acts while doing mathematical problem solving. In D. B. McLeod & V.M. Adam (Eds.), Affect and mathematical problem solving: A new perspective (pp. 117–148). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M. (1995). Cultural–historical psychology: A meso-genetic approach. In L. M. Martin & E. Tobach (Eds.), Social psychology: Theory and practice of doing and knowing (pp. 168–204). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M. (1996). Cultural psychology. Cambridge, MA: Belkrap Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Confrey, J., & Lachance, A. (2000). Transformative teaching experiments through conjecture-driven research design. In A. E. Kelly & R. Lesh (Eds.), Handbook of research design in mathematics and science education (pp. 231–265). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1981). Experience and nature. In J. A. Boydston (Ed.), John Dewey: The later works, 1925–1953, Vol. 1 (pp. 143–197). Carbondale, IL: Southern Illinois University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erickson, F. (1986). Qualitative methods in research on teaching. In M. C. Wittrock (Ed.), The handbook of research on teaching (3rd ed., pp. 119–161). New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forman, E. (1996). sForms of participation in classroom practice: Implications for learning mathematics. In P. Nesher, L. Steffe, P. Cobb, G. Goldin, & B. Greer (Eds.), Theories of mathematical learning (pp. 115–130). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glaser, B. G., & Strauss, A. L. (1967). The discovery of grounded theory: Strategies for qualitative research. New York: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gravemeijer, K. E. P. (1994a). Developing realistic mathematics education. Utrecht, The Netherlands: CD-Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gravemeijer, K. (1994b). Educational development and developmental research. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 25, 443–471.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gravemeijer, K. (1998, April 13). Developmental research: Fostering a dialectic relation between theory and practice. Paper presented the research pPresession of the annual meeting of the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, Washington, DC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gravemeijer, K., Cobb, P., Bowers, J., & Whitenack, J. (2000). Symbolizing, modeling, and instructional design. In P. Cobb, E. Yackel, & K. McClain (Eds.), Symbolizing and communicating in mathematics classrooms: Perspectives on discourse, tools, and instructional design (pp. 225–274). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greeno, J. G. (1991). Number sense as situated knowing in a conceptual domain. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 22, 170–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hatano, G. (1993). Time to merge Vygotskian and constructivist conceptions of knowledge acquisition. In E. A. Forman, N. Minick, & C. A. Stone (Eds.), Contexts for learning: Sociocultural dynamics in children’s development (pp. 153–166). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hiebert, J., & Wearne, D. (1992). Instructional tasks, classroom discourse, and students’ learning in second grade arithmetic. American Educational Research Journal, 30, 393–425.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the wild. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. (1987). The body in the mind: The bodily basis of reason and imagination. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kamii, C. K. (1985). Young children reinvent arithmetic: Implications of Piaget’s theory. New York: Teachers College Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kozulin, A. (1990). Vygotsky’s psychology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lacan, J. (1977). Ecrits: A selection. London: Tavistock.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lampert, M. (1990). When the problem is not the question and the solution is not the answer: Mathematical knowing and teaching. American Educational Research Journal, 27(1), 29–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lampert, M., & Cobb, P. (1998). White paper on communication and language for Standards 2000 Writing Group. Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lave, J. (1988). Cognition in practice: Mind, mathematics and culture in everyday life. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lave, J. (1996). Teaching, as learning, in practice. Mind, Culture, Activity, 3, 149–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral participation. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawler, R. W. (1985). Computer experience and cognitive development: A child’s learning in a computer culture. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lemke, J. L. (1997). Cognition, context, and learning: A semiotic perspective. In D. Kirshner & J. A. Whitson (Eds.), Situated cognition theory: Social, semiotic, and psychological perspectives (pp. 37–56). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lerman, S. (1996). Intersubjectivity in mathematics learning: A challenge to the radical constructivist paradigm? Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 27, 133–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClain, K., & Cobb, P. (1999). Patterning and partitioning: Early number concepts. In J. Copley (Ed.), Mathematics in the early years (pp. 112–118). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics; Washington, DC: National Association for the Education of Young Children.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClain, K., Cobb., P., & Gravemeijer, K. (2000). Supporting students’ ways of reasoning about data. In M. Burke & F. Curcio (Eds.), Learning mathematics for a new century (pp. 174–187). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClain, K., Cobb, P., Gravemeijer, K., & Estes, B. (1999). Developing mathematical reasoning within the context of measurement. In L. Stiff & F. R. Curcio (Eds.), Developing mathematical reasoning in grades K–12 (1999 Yearbook of the Nation Council of Teachers of Mathematics) (pp. 93–106). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meira, L. (1998). Making sense of instructional devices: The emergence of transparency in mathematical activity. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 29, 121–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Minick, N. (1989). L. S. Vygotsky and Soviet activity theory: Perspectives on the relationship between mind and society (Special Monograph Series No. 1). Newton, MA: Educational Development Center, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mishler, E. G. (1986). Research interviewing: Context and narrative. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Much, N. C., & Shweder, R. A. (1978). Speaking of rules: The analysis of culture in breach. New Directions for Child Development, 2, 19–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newman, D., Griffin, P., & Cole, M. (1989). The construction zone: Working for cognitive change in school. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pea, R. D. (1992). Augmenting the discourse of learning with computer based learning environments. In E. de Corte, M. Linn, & L. Verschaffel (Eds.), Computer-based learning environments and problem-solving (pp. 47–87). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pea, R. D. (1993). Practices of distributed intelligence and designs for education. In G. Solomon (Ed.), Distributed cognitions (pp. 47–87). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1970). Genetic epistemology. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1973). To understand is to invent. New York: Grossman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J., Inhelder, B., & Szeminska, A. (1960). The child’s conception of geometry. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B. (1995). Observing sociocultural activity on three planes: Participatory appropriation, guided participation, and apprenticeship. In J. V. Wersch, P. del Rio, & A. Alvarez (Eds.), Sociocultural studies of mind (pp. 139–164). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B. (1997). Evaluating development in the process of perticipation: Theory, methods, and practice building on each other. In E. Amsel & A. Renninger (Eds.), Change and development: Issues of theory, application and method. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rommetveit, R. (1992). Outlines of a dialogically based social-cognitive approach to human cognition and communication. In A. Heen Wold (Ed.), The dialogical alternative: Towards a theory of language and mind (pp. 19–44). Oslo, Norway: Scandinavian University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roschelle, J. (1990, April 19–23). Designing for conversations. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Boston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W. -M. (2001). Situating cognition. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 10, 27–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W. -M., & McGinn, M. K. (1998). Inscriptions: Toward a theory of representing as social practice. Review of Educational Research, 68(1), 35–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saxe, G. B. (1991). Culture and cognitive development: Studies in mathematical understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saxe, G. B. (1994). Studying cognitive development in sociocultural context: The development of a practice-based approach. Mind, culture, and Activity, 1, 135–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schoenfeld, A. H. (1982). Some thoughts on problem-solving research and mathematics education. In F. K. Lester & J. Garofalo (Eds.), Mathematical problem solving: Issues in Research (pp. 27–37). Philadelphia: Philadelphia: Institute Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schoenfeld, A. H. (1983). Beyond the purely cognitive: Belief systems, social cognition, and metacognitions as driving forces in intellectual performance. Cognitive Science, 7, 329–363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schutz, A. (1962). The problem of social reality. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Martinus Nijhoff.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sfard, A. (2000). Symbolizing mathematical reality into being. In P. Cobb, E. Yackel, & K. McClain (Eds.), Symbolizing and communicating in mathematics classrooms: Perspectives on discourse, tools, and instructional design. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shotter, J. (1995). In dialogue: Social constructionism and radical constructivism. In L. P. Steffe & J. Gale (Eds.), Constructivism in education (pp. 41–56). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, M. A. (2000). Research on mathematics teacher development: The teacher development experiment. In A. E. Kelly & R. Lesh (Eds.), Handbook of research design in mathematics and science education (pp. 335–359). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, M. S., & Blume, G. W. (1996). Justification in the mathematics classroom: A study of prospective elementary teachers. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 15, 3–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skemp, R. R. (1982). Theories and methologies. In T. P. Carpenter, J. M. Moser, & T. A. Romberg (Eds.), Addition and subtraction: A cognitive perspective (pp. 18–41). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J. P. (1997). Problems with problematizing mathematics: A reply to Hiebert et al. Educational Researcher, 26(2), 22–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steffe, L. P., & Kieren, T. (1994). Radical constructivism and mathematics education. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 25, 711–733.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stephan, M. L. (1998). Supporting the development of one first-grade classroom’s conceptions of measurement: Analyzing students’ learning in social context. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stone, C. A. (1993). What is missing in the metaphor of scaffolding? In E. A. Forman, N. Minick, & C. A. Stone (Eds.), Contexts for learning: Sociocultural dynamics in children’s development (pp. 169–183). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Streefland, L. (1991). Fractions in realistic mathematics education: A paradigm of developmental research. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. J., & Bogdan, R. (1984). Introduction to qualitative research methods (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, A. G., Philipp, R. A., Thompson, P. W., & Boyd, B. (1994). Calculational and conceptial orientations in teaching mathematics. 1994 Yearbook of the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, A. G., & Thompson, P. W. (1996). Talking about rates conceptually, Part II: Mathematical knowledge for teaching. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 27, 2–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, P. (1994). The development of the concept of speed and its relationship to concepts of rate. In E. Harel & J. Confrey (Eds.), The development of multiplicative reasoning in the learning of mathematics (pp. 181–287). Albany: State University of New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, P. W. (1991). To experience is to conceptualize: A discussion of epistemology and mathematical experience. In L. P. Steffe (Ed.), Epistemological foundations of mathematical experience (pp. 260–281). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, P. W. (1993). Qualitative reasoning, complexity, and additive structures. Educational studies in Mathematics, 25, 165–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toulmin, S. (1969). The uses of argument. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treffers, A. (1987). Three dimensions: A model of goal and theory description in mathematics instruction— The Wiskobas Project. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Reidel.

    Google Scholar 

  • van der Veer, R., & Valsiner, J. (1991). Understanding Vygotsky: A quest for synthesis. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Oers, B. (1996). Learning mathematics as meaningful activity. In P. Nesher, L. Steffe, P. Cobb, G. Goldin, & B. Greer (Eds.), Theories of mathematical learning (pp. 91–114). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Oers, B. (2000). The appropriation of mathematical symbols: A psychosemiotic approach to mathematical learning. In P. Cobb, E. Yackel, & K. McClain (Eds.), Symbolizing and communicating in mathematics classrooms: Perspectives on discourse, tools, and instructional design. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F. J., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (1991). The embodied mind: Cognitive science and human experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Voigt, J. (1995). Thematic patterns of interaction and sociomathematical norms. In P. Cobb & H. Bauersfeld (Eds.), Emergence of mathematical meaning: Interaction in classroom cultures (pp. 163–201). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Voigt, J. (1996). Negotiation of mathematical meaning in classroom processes. In P. Nesher, L. P. Steffe, P. Cobb, G. Goldin, & B. Greer (Eds.), Theories of mathematical learning (pp. 21–50). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walkerdine, V. (1988). The mastery of reason: Cognitive development and the production of rationality. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wertsch, J. V. (1991). Voices of the mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wertsch, J. V. (1994). The primacy of mediated action in sociocultural studies. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 1, 202–208.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winograd, T., & Flores, F. (1986). Understanding computers and cognition: A new foundation for design. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yackel, E. (1995). The classroom teaching experiment. Unpublished manuscript, Purdue University, Department of Mathematical Sciences, Hammond, IN.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yackel, E. (1997, March 24–28). Explanation as an interactive accomplishment: A case study of one second-grade mathematics classroom. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yackel, E., & Cobb, P. (1996). Sociomath norms, argumentation, and autonomy in mathematics. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 27, 458–477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zevenbergen, R. (1996). Constructivism as liberal bourgeois discourse. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 31, 95–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The analysis we report was supported by the National Science Foundation under Grant REC 9814898 and by the Office of Educational Research and Improvement under Grant R305A60007.

We are grateful to Sasha Barab, Joanna Kulikowkich, Geoffrey Saxe, and Michael Young for helpful comments on a previous draft of this article.

The opinions expressed do not necessarily reflect the views of either the Foundation or OERI.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Paul Cobb .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2010 Springer Science+Business Media B.V.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Cobb, P., Stephan, M., McClain, K., Gravemeijer, K. (2010). Participating in Classroom Mathematical Practices. In: Sfard, A., Gravemeijer, K., Yackel, E. (eds) A Journey in Mathematics Education Research. Mathematics Education Library, vol 48. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-90-481-9729-3_9

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics