Skip to main content

Learning from Distributed Theories of Intelligence

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
A Journey in Mathematics Education Research

Part of the book series: Mathematics Education Library ((MELI,volume 48))

  • 1870 Accesses

Abstract

The analysis reported in this article is grounded in the practice of classroom-based developmental or transformational research and focuses on the distributed views of intelligence developed by Pea (1993) and by Hutchins (1995). The general areas of agreement with this theoretical perspective include both the nondualist orientation and the critical role attributed to tool use. Against this background, I focus on two aspects of the distributed view that I and my colleagues have found necessary to modify for our purposes. The first concerns the legitimacy of taking the individual as the unit of analysis, and here I argue that the distributed view implicitly accepts key tenets of mainstream American psychology’s characterization of the individual even as it explicitly rejects it. The second modification concerns distributed intelligence’s characterization of tool use. Drawing on a distinction made by Dewey, I argue that it is more useful for the purposes of instructional design to focus on activity that involves using the tool as an instrument, rather than focusing on the tool itself.

Mind, Culture, and Activity, 5 (1998), 187–204.

Copyright © 1998, Regents of the University of California on behalf of the Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 139.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. 1.

    I repeatedly use the first-person plural to acknowledge the collaborative nature of our research. From 1986 until 1991, these colleagues were Ema Yackel and Terry Wood, and from 1991 until the present they are Ema Yackel, Koeno Gravemeijer, Janet Bowers, Kay McClain, and Joy Whitenack.

  2. 2.

    These provisional sequences provide an initial orientation for the teaching experiment. Adaptations and modifications are made on a daily basis throughout the experiment so that the actual sequences realized in the classroom typically differ significantly from those envisioned at the outset.

  3. 3.

    Coming to reason with groups of beads rather than counting beads one by one is itself a developmental achievement for young children. We supported the development of these ways of reasoning in the teaching experiment by designing an instructional sequence called Patterning and Partitioning. This sequence was enacted in the classroom immediately before the arithmetic rack was introduced.

  4. 4.

    Psychological analyses almost universally report a developmental progression of increasingly sophisticated counting methods (e.g., counting all and counting on) followed by thinking strategies. The purview of these analyses is, of course, restricted to students who participate in currently institutionalized classroom practices.

  5. 5.

    Although atypical in the United States, this approach of monitoring students’ activity to plan for the subsequent whole-class discussion is routine in Japan (Stigler, Fernandez, & Yoshida, 1996).

  6. 6.

    Judgments of mathematical significance are made with respect to current conjectures about the classroom community’s learning trajectory and the means of supporting it. In other words, although goals and conjectures continue to evolve throughout the experiment, one has in mind an envisioned learning trajectory at any particular point in the experiment. The currently anticipated learning trajectory both provides a sense of direction and constitutes the broader setting in which judgments of mathematical significance are made. In particular, an issue is judged to be mathematically significant if it contributes to the realization of the currently envisioned learning trajectory. Our observations of the subsequent whole-class discussion can, however, lead us to revise this judgment and to modify the conjectured learning trajectory. As a consequence, the actual learning trajectory realized in the classroom is enacted jointly by the students, the teacher, and the researchers. To paraphrase Varela, Thompson, and Rosch (1991), it is much like a path that exists only as it is laid down by walking, even though we have a sense of where we are going and how we might get there at each moment.

  7. 7.

    This is particularly the case with Hutchins (1995), whose book is directed primarily toward cognitive scientists.

  8. 8.

    Some years ago, we described this relation as dialectical. However, German colleagues noted that, in their country, the use of the term dialectical is often viewed as indicating a commitment to the philosophy of dialectical materialism. To avoid such confusions, we prefer to speak of reflexive rather than dialectical relations.

  9. 9.

    It is important to stress that the issue at hand is not that of coordinating two sets of separate processes—one psychological and the other communal. Instead, we coordinate different ways of interpreting and describing classroom activity.

  10. 10.

    As stated at the outset, the interests that motivate this discussion are those of instructional design and reform at the classroom level. It should however be acknowledged that mathematical knowing is social through and through in a second sense. In particular, generally accepted beliefs about what counts as normal in development and as more and less sophisticated are themselves social constructions that are reflexively verified in practice (Cobb & Yackel, 1996; Walkerdine, 1988).

  11. 11.

    In doing so, we took a psychological perspective and focused on individual students’ qualitatively distinct ways of participating in classroom mathematical practices. In contrast, we adopted a social perspective when we planned the instructional sequence and envisioned the evolution of taken-as-shared, communal ways of reasoning with the arithmetic rack.

  12. 12.

    Strictly speaking, this focus on experience is redundant: The world acted in is the world experienced. I have used the term experience to differentiate the world-acted-in from what an observer might take to be the environment that can be analyzed independently of activity in propositional terms.

  13. 13.

    Critiques of IP psychology that elaborate the contrast between the actor’s and the observer’s viewpoints can be found in Cobb (1987, 1990).

References

  • Bateson, G. (1973). Steps to an ecology of mind. London: Paladin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bednarz, N., Dufour-Janvier, B., Poirier, L., & Bacon, L. (1993). Socio constructivist viewpoint on the use of symbolism in mathematics education. Alberta Journal of Educational Research, 39, 41–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, J., Cobb, P., & McClain, K. (1999). The evolution of mathematical practices: A case study. Cognition and Instruction.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cobb, P. (1987). Information-processing psychology and mathematics education—A constructivist perspective. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 6, 3–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cobb, P. (1990). A constructivist perspective on information-processing theories of mathematics education. International Journal of Educational Research, 14, 67–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cobb, P. (1996, July). Accounting for mathematical learning in the social context of the classroom. Paper presented at the Eighth International Congress on Mathematical Education, Seville, Spain.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cobb, P. (2000). Conducting classroom teaching experiments in collaboration with teachers. In A. E. Kelly & R. Lesh (Eds.), Handbook of research design in mathematics and science education (pp. 307–334). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cobb, P., Gravemeijer, K., Yackel, E., McClain, K., & Whitenack, J. (1997). Mathematizing and symbolizing: The emergence of chains of signification in one first-grade classroom. In D. Kirshner & J. A. Whitson (Eds.), Situated cognition theory: Social, semiotic, and neurological perspectives (pp. 151–233). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cobb, P., & Yackel, E. (1996). Constructivist, emergent, and sociocultural perspectives in the context of developmental KS&sadn. Educational Psychologist, 31, 175–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M., & Engeström, Y. (1993). A cultural-historical approach to distributed cognition. In G. Salomon (Ed.), Distributed cognitions (pp. 1–6). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Confrey, J., & Lachance, A. (2000). Transformative teaching experiments through conjecture-driven research design. In A. E. Kelly & R. Lesh (Eds.), Handbook of research design in mathematics and science education (pp. 231–265). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1977). In J. A. Boydston (Ed.), John Dewey, The middle works. 1899–1924 (Vol. 2, pp. 362–363). Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dörfler, W. (1989). Protocols of actions as a cognitive tool for knowledge construction. In G. Vergnaud, J. Rogalski, & M. Artigue (Eds.), Proceedings of the Thirteenth Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (pp. 212–219). Paris, France: PME.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dörfler, W. (1993). Computer use and views of the mind. In C. Keitel & K. Ruthven (Eds.), Learning from computers: Mathematics education and technology (pp. 159–186). Berlin: Springer Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fuson, K. C. (1992). Research on whole number addition and subtraction. In D. A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 243–275). New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gravemeijer, K. E. P. (1994). Developing realistic mathematics education. Utrecht, The Netherlands: CD-p Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gravemeijer, K., Cobb, P., Bowers, J. & Whitenack, J. (2000). Symbolizing, modeling, and instructional design. In P. Cobb, E. Yackel, & K. McClain (Eds.), Symbolizing and communicating in mathematics classrooms: Perspectives on discourse, tools, and instructional design. (pp. 225–274) Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greeno, J. G. (1991). Number sense as situated knowing in a conceptual domain. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 22, 170–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hatano, G. (1993). Time to merge Vygotskian and constructivist conceptions of knowledge acquisition. In E. A. Forman, N. Minick, & C. A. Stone (Eds.), Contexts for learning: Sociocultural dynamics in children’s development (pp. 153–166). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the wild. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins, E. (1996). Response to reviewers. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 3, 64–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • John-Steiner, V. (1995). Spontaneous and scientific concepts in mathematics: A Vygotskian approach. In L. Meira & D. Carraher (Eds.), Proceedings of the 19th International Conference for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (Vol. 1, pp. 30–44). Recife, Brazil: Program Committee of 19th PME Conference.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. (1987). The body in the mind: The bodily basis of reason and imagination. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaput, J. J. (1987). Towards a theory of symbol use in mathematics. In C. Janvier (Ed.), Problems of representation in the teaching and learning of mathematics (pp. 159–195). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaput, J. J. (1991). Notations and representations as mediators of constructive processes. In E. von Glasersfeld (Ed.), Constructivism in mathematics education (pp. 53–74). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B. (1996). Review of Cognition in the Wild. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 3, 54–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lave, J. (1993). Word problems: A microcosm of theories of learning. In P. Light & G. Butterworth (Eds.), Context and cognition: Ways of learning and knowing (pp. 74–92). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lemke, J. L. (1997). Cognition, context, and learning: A social semiotic perspective. In D. Kirshner & J. A. Whitson (Eds.), Situated cognition theory: Social, semiotic, and neurological perspectives (pp. 37–55). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mason, J. (1987). What do symbols represent? In C. Janvier (Ed.), Problems of representation in the learning and teaching of mathematics (pp. 73–82). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mehan, H., & Wood, H. (1975). The reality of ethnomethodology. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meira, L. (1995). The microevolution of mathematical representations in children’s activity. Cognition and Instruction, 13, 269–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nemirovsky, R. (1994). On ways of symbolizing: The case of Laura and the velocity sign. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 13(4), 389–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nunes, T., Schliemann, A. D., & Carraher, D. W. (1993). Street mathematics and school mathematics. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pea, R. D. (1985). Beyond amplification: Using computers to reorganize human mental functioning. Educational Psychologist, 20, 167–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pea, R. D. (1987). Cognitive technologies for mathematics education. In A. H. Schoenfeld (Ed.), Cognitive science and mathematics education (pp. 89–122). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pea, R. D. (1992). Augmenting the discourse of learning with computer-based learning environments. In E. de Corte, M. Linn, & L. Verschaffel (Eds.), Computer-based learning environments and problem solving (NATO Series, Subseries F: Computer and Systems Sciences, pp. 313–343). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pea, R. D. (1993). Practices of distributed intelligence and designs for education. In G. Salomon (Ed.), Distributed cognitions (pp. 47–87). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1970). Genetic epistemology. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pirie, S., & Kieran, T. (1994). Growth in mathematical understanding: How can we characterize it and how can we represent it? Educational Studies in Mathematics, 26, 61–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prawat, R. S. (1995). Misreading Dewey: Reform, projects, and the language game. Educational Researcher, 24(7), 13–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B. (1995). Observing sociocultural activity on three planes: Participatory appropriation, guided participation, and apprenticeship. In J. V. Wertsch, P. del Rio, & A. Alvarez (Eds.), Sociocultural studies of mind (pp. 139–164). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rorrunetveit, R. (1992). Outlines of adialogically based social-cognitive approach to human cognition and communication. In A. Heen Wold (Ed.), The dialogical alternative: Towards a theory of language and mind (pp. 19–44). Oslo, Norway: Scandinavian University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salomon, G. (1993). No distribution without individuals’ cognition: A dynamic interactional view. In G. Salomon (Ed.), Distributed cognitions (pp. 111–138). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saxe, G. B. (1991). Culture and cognitive development: Studies in mathematical understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schutz, A. (1962). The problem of social reality. The Hague, The Netherlands: Martinus Nijhoff.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sfard, A., & Linchevski, L. (1994). The gains and pitfalls of reification—the case of algebra. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 26, 87–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shotter, J. (1995). In dialogue: Social constructionism and radical constructivism. In L. P. Steffe & J. Gale (Eds.), Constructivism in education (pp. 41–56). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steffe, L. P., Cobb, P., & von Glasersfeld, E. (1988). Construction of arithmetical meanings and strategies. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stigler, J. W., Femandez, C, & Yoshida, M. (1996). Traditions of school mathematics in Japanese and American elementary classrooms. In L. P. Steffe, P. Nesher, P. Cobb, G. Goldin, & B. Greer (Eds.), Theories of mathematical learning (pp. 149–175). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Streefiand, L. (1991). Fractions in realistic mathematics education: A paradigm of developmental research. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, P. W. (1992). Notations, principles, and constraints: Contributions to the effective use of concrete manipulatives in elementary mathematics. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 23, 123–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treffers, A. (1987). Three dimensions: A model of goal and theory description in mathematics instructionThe Wiskobas Project. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Reidel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treffers, A. (1990). Rekenen tot twintig met het rekeruek [Addition and subtraction up to twenty with the arithmetic rack]. WillemBartjens, 10(1), 35–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ueno, N. (1995). The social construction of reality in the artifacts of numeracy for distribution and exchange in a Nepalese bazaar. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 2, 240–257.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Oers, B. (1996). Leaming mathematics as meaningful activity. In P. Nesher, L. Steffe, P. Cobb, G. Goldin, & B. Greer (Eds.), Theories of mathematical learning (pp. 91–114). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F. J., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (1991). The embodied mind: Cognitive science and human experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Glasersfeld, E. (1991). Abstraction, representation, and reflection: An interpretation of experience and Piaget’s approach. In L. P. Steffe (Ed.), Epistemological foundations of mathematical experience (pp. 45–67). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). Thinking and speech. In R. W. Rieber & A. S. Carton (Eds.), The collected works of Vygotsky, L. S. Vol. 1: Problems of general psychology (pp. 37–285). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walkerdine, V. (1988). The mastery of reason. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wertsch, J. V. (1994a). The primacy of mediated action in sociocultural studies. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 1, 202–208.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wertsch, J. V. (1994b). Struggling with the past: Some dynamics of historical representation. In M. Carretero & J. Voss (Eds.), Cognitive and instructional processes in history and the social sciences (pp. 323–338). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitenack, J. W. (1995). Modeling, mathematizing, and mathematical learning as it is situated in the classroom microculture. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitson, J. A. (1997). Cognition as a semiosic process: Grounding, mediation, and critical reflective transcendence. In D. Kirshner & J. A. Whitson (Eds.), Situated cognition theory: Social, semiotic, and neurological perspectives (pp. 97–149). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winograd, T., & Flores, F. (1986). Understanding computers and cognition: A new foundation for design. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yackel, E. (1995). The classroom teaching experiment. Unpublished manuscript, Purdue University, Calumet, IL.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The analysis reported in this article was supported by the National Science Foundation under Grant RED-9353587 and by the Office of Educational Research and Improvement under Grant R305A60007. The opinions expressed do not necessarily reflect the views of either the Foundation or of OERI.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Paul Cobb .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2010 Springer Science+Business Media B.V.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Cobb, P. (2010). Learning from Distributed Theories of Intelligence. In: Sfard, A., Gravemeijer, K., Yackel, E. (eds) A Journey in Mathematics Education Research. Mathematics Education Library, vol 48. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-90-481-9729-3_7

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics