Skip to main content

Lost in Space: Consciousness and Experiment in the Work of Irwin and Turrell

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Beyond Mimesis and Convention

Part of the book series: Boston Studies in the Philosophy of Science ((BSPS,volume 262))

  • 1697 Accesses

Abstract

On several occasions during the years 1968–1971 artists Robert Irwin and James Turrell, an experimental psychologist named Ed Wortz, and a number of UCLA student volunteers spent hours depriving themselves of light, sound and human contact. They were engaged in a series of experiments involving an anechoic chamber used for psycho-physical experimentation by the Garrett Corporation, a contractor to the National Aeronautics and Space Agency (NASA). The interior of the chamber was soundproofed, suspended to minimize the effects of the earth’s rotation and utterly darkened. Self-projected sounds like speech were deadened. Sitting in these reduced surroundings was exhausting; rather than depriving the subject of the senses of sight and hearing, the lack of focal markers proved to heighten them, causing the subject to strain his eyes and ears, searching for something upon which to focus his attention. Most startling were the effects upon leaving the chamber when the body re-adjusted to the overwhelming array of stimuli in daily life and the world became intensely bright, loud and noticeable.

[A]part from the experiences of subjects there is nothing, nothing, nothing, bare nothingness.

Alfred North Whitehead, 1929

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    “Bracketing” (Einklammerung) is a term first posited in Edmund Husserl (1991, original 1913). The term is succinctly defined by Husserl scholar David Woodruff Smith (2007, 429) as “the method or technique of turning our attention from the objects of our consciousness to our consciousness of those objects”, an awareness of being aware, so to speak.

  2. 2.

    This is my primary reason for not including images with this text. While I acknowledge that my own descriptions are limited, they are less likely to be mistaken for “the thing itself” than are photographs, which at best offer only severely limited versions of the work in question, and prioritize visual perception at the expense of other sensory percepts present in the immediate experience. For years, Robert Irwin held the same position and refused to allow his work to be photographed, though he has since relented.

  3. 3.

    Robert Irwin, quoted in Weschler (1982, 99).

  4. 4.

    Davis (1992, 54–62); and Tuchman (1971). For more extensive histories on the development and presence of the scientific community and specifically the aerospace industry in Southern California see: Newell (1980) and Koppes (1982), whose work relies upon an in-depth knowledge of the inner workings of the Jet Propulsion Laboratory as gleaned from its (de-classified) records.

  5. 5.

    Some have interpreted this proposal as especially disturbing and manipulative, seeing the viewers as playing the part of unsuspecting test subjects (Perchuk 2006). Though the experience would likely have been discomfiting, I can’t agree entirely. Considering that the option to participate would have been solely the viewer’s, and that no evidence indicates that the results would be “classified” or that one viewer would be prevented from discussing the procedure with the next, participants could hardly accuse the artists—who if anything were looking for ways to communicate the experience—of coercive tactics.

  6. 6.

    Joseph Kosuth, quoted in Alberro and Stimson (1999, xxxi n. 7).

  7. 7.

    The laboratory analogy is by no means isolated, nor was it particularly new by the middle of the twentieth century. In 1905, for example, the trustees of the Boston Museum of Art specially commissioned an experimental gallery for the purpose of testing conditions especially lighting conditions in a scientific manner (Gilman 1905, 1906). Excessive glare and shadow especially were to be avoided. The Boston trustees’ prejudice in favor of the eye (rather than brain) as the critical viewing organ continued in the preparation of gallery spaces elsewhere in twentieth-century America. Seventy years later Brian O’Doherty (1976, 29) asserted how this prejudice had come to dominate curatorial practice: “It is now impossible to paint up an exhibition without surveying the wall like a health inspector …” The irony of discussing these experiments in this context is that light and space art often consists of nothing but glare and/or shadow.

  8. 8.

    James Turrell, in conversation with Jan Butterfield in Butterfield (1993, 69–71); Turrell (1980, 27–29).

References

  • Adcock, C., (ed.) (1990), James Turrell: The Art of Light and Space. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alberro, A. and Stimson, B. (eds.) (1999), Conceptual Art: A Critical Anthology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baars, B. (1997), In the Theater of Consciousness. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Berthoz, A. (2000), The Brain’s Sense of Movement. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blackmore, S. (2006), Conversations on Consciousness. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butterfield, J. (1993), The Art of Light and Space. New York: Abbeville Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cage, J. (1961), Silence: Lectures and Writings by John Cage. Hanover, NH: Wesleyan University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Churchland, P. (1986), Neurophilosophy. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Compton, M. (1970), Larry Bell, Robert Irwin, Doug Wheeler. London: Tate Gallery of Art.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. (1999), The Feeling of What Happens. New York: Harcourt, Brace & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, M. (1992), City of Quartz. New York: Vintage Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1934), Art as Experience. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Emmerik, P. V., in collaboration with Herbert Henck and András Wilheim (2003–2007), A John Cage Compendium. http://www.xs4all.nl/~cagecomp/

  • Flores-González, L. M. (2008), “Phenomenological Views on Intersubjectivity: Towards a Reinterpretation of Consciousness”, Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science 42: 187–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fried, M. (1964), “New York Letter”, Art International 8: 81–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fried, M. (1967), “Art and Objecthood”, reprinted in Michael Fried (1998), Art and Objecthood. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert-Rolfe, J. (1993), “Expression: Lines, Dots, Discs; Light”, in R. Ferguson (ed.), Robert Irwin. Los Angeles: Museum of Contemporary Art, Los Angeles, NY: Rizzoli Publications, 93–111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilman, B. (1905), “The Museum Commission to Europe”, Communications to the Trustees, III. Boston, MA: Boston Museum of Fine Arts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilman, B. (1906), “The Experimental Gallery”, Communications to the Trustees, IV. Boston, MA: Boston Museum of Fine Arts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, C. (1962), “After Abstract Expression”, Art International 1: 24–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamlyn, D. W. (1961), Sensation and Perception. New York: Humanities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamlyn, D. W. (1990), In and Out of the Black Box: on the Philosophy of Cognition. London: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, E. (1991), Ideas Pertaining to a Pure Phenomenology and a Phenomenological Philosophy, First Book: General Introduction to a Pure Phenomenology, trans. Fred Kersten. Dordrecht and Boston, MA: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Irwin, R. (1970–2004), Robert Irwin Papers, Getty Research Institute, Research Library, Accession no. 940081.

    Google Scholar 

  • Irwin, R. (1977), Robert Irwin. New York: Whitney Museum of Art.

    Google Scholar 

  • Irwin, R. (1985), Being and Circumstance. Larkspur Landing, CA: The Lapis Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1890), Principles of Psychology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. (2007), The Meaning of the Body. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Judd, D. (1969), “Complaints: Part I”, Studio International 177: 166+.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koppes, C. R. (1982), JPL and the American Space Program. New Haven, CT and London: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leider, P. (1966), Robert Irwin. Los Angeles, CA: Los Angeles County Museum of Art.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, P. (2006), Chronophobia: on Time in the Art of the 1960s. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Magnifico, M. and Lucia B. D. (2001), Villas Menafoglio Litta Panza and the Panza di Biuomo Collection. Geneva, Milan: Fondo per L’Ambiente Italiano.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G. (1975), “Consciousness: Respectable, Useful, and Probably Necessary”, in R. L. Solso (ed.), Informational Processing and Cognition: The Loyola Symposium, New Jersey: Erlbaum, 229–254.

    Google Scholar 

  • Metzinger, T. (2003), Being No One. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A. (2003), “The Cognitive Revolution: A Historical Perspective”, Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7: 141–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U. (1967), Cognitive Psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, H. (1980), Beyond the Atmosphere: Early Years of Space Science. Washington, DC: Scientific and Technical Information Branch, National Aeronautics and Space Administration.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newman, A. (2000), Challenging Art: Artforum 1962–1974. New York: Soho Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Odenwald, S. F. (2002), Patterns in the Void. New York: Westview Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Doherty, B. (1976), Inside the White Cube (expanded edition, 1986). San Francisco, CA: Lapis Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, S. E. (1999), Vision Science: Photons to Phenomenology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perchuk, A. (2006), “From Otis to Ferus: Robert Irwin, Ed Ruscha, and Peter Voulkos in Los Angeles”, Ph.D. dissertation, Yale University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petitot, J., Varela, F. J., Pachoud, B. and Roy, J.-M. (eds.) (1999), Naturalizing Phenomenology: Issues in Contemporary Phenomenology and Cognitive Science. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (2002), The Blank Slate. New York: Viking Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robertson, W. G. and Wortz, E. C. (1969), The Effects of Lunar Gravity on Metabolic Rates. Washington, DC: National Aeronautics and Space Agency.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schrödinger, E. (1935), “The Present Situation in Quantum Mechanics”, trans. J. D. Timmer, Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society 124: 323–338.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1964), “Man”, Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society 108: 482–485.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1974), About Behaviorism. New York: Alfred A. Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, D. W. (2007), Husserl. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, E., Lutz, A., and Cosmelli, D. (2005), “Neurophenomenology: An Introduction for Neurophilosophers”, in Andrew Brook and Kathleen Akins (eds.), Cognition and the Brain: The Philosophy and Neuroscience Movement, Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 40–97.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tuchman, M. and Livingston, J. (1971), A Report on the Art & Technology Program of the Los Angeles County Museum of Art 1967–1971. New York: Viking Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turrell, J. (1980), James Turrell, Light and Space. New York: Whitney Museum of American Art.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F. J. (1996), “Neurophenomenology: A Methodological Remedy for the Hard Problem”, Journal of Consciousness Studies 3: 330–350.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vitz, P. C. and A. Glimcher (1984), Modern Art and Modern Science: The Parallel Analysis of Vision. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B. (1925, rev. ed. 1930), Behaviorism. New York: W.W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weschler, L. (1982), Seeing is Forgetting the Name of the Thing One Sees. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whiting, C. (2006), Pop L.A.: Art and the City in the 1960s. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Dawna Schuld .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2010 Springer Science+Business Media B.V.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Schuld, D. (2010). Lost in Space: Consciousness and Experiment in the Work of Irwin and Turrell. In: Frigg, R., Hunter, M. (eds) Beyond Mimesis and Convention. Boston Studies in the Philosophy of Science, vol 262. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-90-481-3851-7_10

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics