Skip to main content

Experimental Studies of Modern Human Social and Individual Learning in an Archaeological Context: People Behave Adaptively, But Within Limits

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Dynamics of Learning in Neanderthals and Modern Humans Volume 2

Part of the book series: Replacement of Neanderthals by Modern Humans Series ((RNMH))

Abstract

It has been proposed that one reason for the success of Homo sapiens is our advanced learning abilities. Theoretical models suggest that complex cultural adaptations can arise from an optimal mix of (1) individual learning that is of sufficient accuracy plus (2) social learning that is of sufficiently high fidelity and is payoff-biased. Here I review the findings of a series of experimental studies of human learning, designed to simulate the kind of technology-based tasks that our ancestors would have faced. Results of these studies support the predictions of the models, and show that contemporary humans’ learning strategies are broadly adaptive. Performance typically improves through effective individual learning and payoff-biased social learning. The latter crucially allows participants to escape low-fitness locally optimal artifact designs and jump to higher-fitness designs, assuming a realistic multimodal adaptive landscape underlying artifact fitness. On the other hand, people also exhibited predictable flaws in their learning, such as the copying of neutral traits exhibited by successful models along with their functional traits (i.e., cultural hitchhiking), and an unwillingness to share information with others under certain circumstances.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Akazawa T (ed) (2012) Replacement of Neanderthals by modern humans (RNMH) project series, vol 1. RNMH Project Group, Kochi University of Technology, Tokyo

    Google Scholar 

  • Aoki K, Wakano JY, Feldman MW (2005) The emergence of social learning in a temporally changing environment: a theoretical model. Curr Anthropol 46:334–340

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aoki K, Lehmann L, Feldman MW (2011) Rates of cultural change and patterns of cultural accumulation in stochastic models of social transmission. Theor Popul Biol 79:192–202

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aoki K, Wakano JY, Lehmann L (2012) Evolutionarily stable learning schedules and cumulative culture in discrete generation models. Theor Popul Biol 81:300–309

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atkisson C, Mesoudi A, O’Brien MJ (2012) Adult learners in a novel environment use prestige-biased social learning. Evol Psychol 10:519–537

    Google Scholar 

  • Bentley RA, Hahn MW, Shennan SJ (2004) Random drift and culture change. Proc R Soc B 271:1443–1450

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bettinger RL, Eerkens J (1999) Point typologies, cultural transmission, and the spread of bow-and-arrow technology in the prehistoric Great Basin. Am Antiq 64:231–242

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd R, Richerson PJ (1985) Culture and the evolutionary process. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd R, Richerson PJ (1988) An evolutionary model of social learning: the effects of spatial and temporal variation. In: Zentall T, Galef BG (eds) Social learning. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 29–48

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd R, Richerson PJ (1992) How microevolutionary processes give rise to history. In: Nitecki M, Nitecki DV (eds) History and evolution. State University of New York Press, Albany, pp 178–209

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd R, Richerson PJ (1995) Why does culture increase human adaptability? Ethol Sociobiol 16:125–143

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cavalli-Sforza LL, Feldman MW (1981) Cultural transmission and evolution. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheshier J, Kelly RL (2006) Projectile point shape and durability: the effect of thickness: length. Am Antiq 71:353–363

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dean LG, Kendal RL, Schapiro SJ, Thierry B, Laland KN (2012) Identification of the social and cognitive processes underlying human cumulative culture. Science 335:1114–1118

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunnell RC (1978) Style and function: a fundamental dichotomy. Am Antiq 35:305–319

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Durham WH (1991) Coevolution: genes, culture, and human diversity. Stanford University Press, Stanford

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehn M, Laland KN (2012) Adaptive strategies for cumulative cultural learning. J Theor Biol 301:103–111

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Enquist M, Ghirlanda S, Jarrick A, Wachtmeister CA (2008) Why does human culture increase exponentially? Theor Popul Biol 74:46–55

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gergely G, Csibra G (2006) Sylvia’s recipe: the role of imitation and pedagogy in the transmission of cultural knowledge. In: Enfield NJ, Levenson SC (eds) Roots of human sociality: culture, cognition, and human interaction. Berg, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J, Boyd R (1998) The evolution of conformist transmission and the emergence of between-group differences. Evol Hum Behav 19:215–241

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J, Gil-White FJ (2001) The evolution of prestige. Evol Hum Behav 22:165–196

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill K, Barton M, Hurtado AM (2009) The emergence of human uniqueness. Evol Anthropol 18:187–200

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hrubesch C, Preuschoft S, van Schaik C (2009) Skill mastery inhibits adoption of observed alternative solutions among chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Anim Cogn 12:209–216

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jensen K, Call J, Tomasello M (2007) Chimpanzees are rational maximizers in an ultimatum game. Science 318:107–109

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laland KN (2004) Social learning strategies. Learn Behav 32:4–14

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lycett SJ, von Cramon-Taubadel N (2008) Acheulean variability and hominin dispersals: a model-bound approach. J Archaeol Sci 35:553–562

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marshall-Pescini S, Whiten A (2008) Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and the question of cumulative culture: an experimental approach. Anim Cogn 11:449–456

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McElreath R, Lubell M, Richerson PJ, Waring TM, Baum W, Edsten E, Efferson C, Paciotti B (2005) Applying evolutionary models to the laboratory study of social learning. Evol Hum Behav 26:483–508

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi A (2007) Using the methods of social psychology to study cultural evolution. J Soc Evol Cult Psychol 1:35–58

    Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi A (2008) An experimental simulation of the ‘copy-successful-individuals’ cultural learning strategy: Adaptive landscapes, producer-scrounger dynamics and informational access costs. Evol Hum Behav 29:350–363

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi A (2009) The cultural dynamics of copycat suicide. PLoS One 4:e7252

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi A (2011a) An experimental comparison of human social learning strategies: payoff-biased social learning is adaptive but underused. Evol Hum Behav 32:334–342

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi A (2011b) Variable cultural acquisition costs constrain cumulative cultural evolution. PLoS One 6:e18239

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi A, Jensen K (2012) Culture and the evolution of human sociality. In: Vonk J, Shackelford T (eds) The oxford handbook of comparative evolutionary psychology. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 419–433

    Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi A, O’Brien MJ (2008a) The cultural transmission of Great Basin projectile point technology I: an experimental simulation. Am Antiq 73:3–28

    Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi A, O’Brien MJ (2008b) The cultural transmission of Great Basin projectile point technology II: an agent-based computer simulation. Am Antiq 73:627–644

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan TJH, Rendell LE, Ehn M, Hoppitt W, Laland KN (2012) The evolutionary basis of human social learning. Proc R Soc B 279:653–662

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neiman FD (1995) Stylistic variation in evolutionary perspective - inferences from decorative diversity and interassemblage distance in Illinois woodland ceramic assemblages. Am Antiq 60:7–36

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien MJ, Darwent J, Lyman RL (2001) Cladistics is useful for reconstructing archaeological phylogenies: Palaeoindian points from the southeastern United States. J Archaeol Sci 28:1115–1136

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Powell A, Shennan S, Thomas MG (2009) Late Pleistocene demography and the appearance of modern human behavior. Science 324:1298–1301

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richerson PJ, Boyd R (2005) Not by genes alone. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers AR (1988) Does biology constrain culture? Am Anthropol 90:819–831

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers DS, Ehrlich PR (2008) Natural selection and cultural rates of change. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105:3416

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silk JB, Brosnan SF, Vonk J, Henrich J, Povinelli DJ, Richardson AS, Lambeth SP, Mascaro J, Schapiro SJ (2005) Chimpanzees are indifferent to the welfare of unrelated group members. Nature 437:1357–1359

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simon HA (1956) Rational choice and the structure of the environment. Psychol Rev 63:129–138

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sperber D (1996) Explaining culture: a naturalistic approach. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Stout D (2002) Skill and cognition in stone tool production. Curr Anthropol 43:693–722

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tennie C, Call J, Tomasello M (2009) Ratcheting up the ratchet: on the evolution of cumulative culture. Philos Trans R Soc B 364:2405–2415

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wright S (1932) The roles of mutation, inbreeding, crossbreeding and selection in evolution. Proceedings of the sixth international congress of genetics 1:356–366

    Google Scholar 

  • Yamamoto S, Humle T, Tanaka M (2013) Basis for cumulative cultural evolution in chimpanzees: social learning of a more efficient tool-use technique. PLoS One 8:e55768

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Alex Mesoudi .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2014 Springer Japan

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Mesoudi, A. (2014). Experimental Studies of Modern Human Social and Individual Learning in an Archaeological Context: People Behave Adaptively, But Within Limits. In: Akazawa, T., Ogihara, N., C Tanabe, H., Terashima, H. (eds) Dynamics of Learning in Neanderthals and Modern Humans Volume 2. Replacement of Neanderthals by Modern Humans Series. Springer, Tokyo. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-4-431-54553-8_8

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics