Skip to main content

Aufmerksamkeit und Bewusstsein

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Allgemeine Psychologie

Zusammenfassung

Kapitel 5 ist ein weiteres klassisches Feld der Allgemeinen Psychologie, das die Fragen nach der Steuerung und Selektivität unserer Aufmerksamkeit (als Teilaspekt der Wahrnehmung) wie auch unser Wissen um unterschiedliche, veränderte oder krankhafte Bewusstseinszustände und Schlaf umfasst. Wir lernen die verschiedenen Mechanismen (und neuropsychologischen Korrelate) der Aufmerksamkeitssteuerung und Selektion sowie die Rolle von Bedürfnissen und Wertungen kennen. Ausführlich werden die verschiedenen Facetten des menschlichen Bewusstseins besprochen und das Problem der Bewusstwerdung erörtert. Dazu kommt abschließend eine ausführlichere Darstellung der neueren Kenntnisse über Funktion und Bedeutung des Schlafes, der zirkadianen Rhythmik (auch unseres Bewusstseins und der Aufmerksamkeit) und des Traumes.

In the simplest terms, „attention“ refers to a selectivity of response. Man or animal is continuously responding to some events in the environment, and not to others that could be responded to (or „noticed“) just as well. When an experimental result makes it necessary to refer to […] „attention“, the reference means, precisely, that the activity that controls the form, speed, strength, or duration of response is not the immediately preceding excitation of receptor cells alone. (Hebb 1949, S. 4)

Nothing is more familiar than the mind […] The first quandary involves the perspective one must adopt to study the conscious mind in relation to the brain in which we believe it originates. Anyone’s body and brain are observable to third parties; the mind, though, is observable only to its owner. (Damasio 1999, S. 114)

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 44.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 59.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Literatur

  • Andersen, S. K., Müller, M. M., & Hillyard, S. A. (2009). Color-selective attention need not be mediated by spatial attention. Journal of Vision, 9, 1–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, E. (1942). Abnormal mental states in survivors, with special reference to collective hallucinations. J. Roy. Nav. Med. Serv., 28, 361–377.

    Google Scholar 

  • Antrobus, J. (1991). Dreaming: Cognitive processes during cortical activation and high afferent thresholds. Psychological Review, 98(1), 96–121.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aschoff, J., & Wever, R. (1966). Circadian period and phase-angle difference in chaffinches (Fringilla coelebs). Comparative Biochemical Physiology, 3, 97–104.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aserinsky, E., & Kleitman, N. (1953). Regulary occuring periods of eye motility, and concomitant phenomena, during sleep. Science, 118, 273–274.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ashby, W. R. (1956). Design for an intelligence amplifier. Automata studies. Princeton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ashby, W. R. (1973). An Introduction to Cybernetics. London: Beccles and Colchester.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ashley, W. R., Harper, R. S., & Runyon, D. L. (1951). The perceived size of coins in normal and hypnotically induced economic states. American Journal of Psychology, 64, 564–572.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, R. L., Atkinson, R. C., Hilgard, E. R. et al. (19531// 1983,8 /200013). (Hilgard’s) Introduction to Psychology. New York, San Diego, Chicago, San Francisco, Atlanta: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Inc. (deutsch 2001, Hilgards Einführung in die Psychologie. Spektrum: Akademischer Verlag).

    Google Scholar 

  • Awh, E., & Pashler, H. (2000). Evidence for split attentional foci. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 834–846.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bacon, W. F., & Egeth, H. E. (1994). Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture. Perception & Psychophysics, 55, 485–496.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. (2007). Social psychological approaches to consciousness. In P. D. Zelazo, M. Moscovich & E. Thompson (Hrsg.), The Cambridge handbook of consciousness (S. 555–570). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Baylis, G. C., & Driver, J. (1993). Visual attention and objects: Evidence for hierarchical coding of location. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 451–470.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Becker, S. I., Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. W. (2013). Attentional capture does not depend on feature similarity, but on target-nontarget relations. Psychological Science, 24, 634–647.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Becker-Carus, C. (1976). Physiologische Psychologie. Psychophysiologische Methodik. In F. Dorsch (Hrsg.), Psychologisches Wörterbuch. Stuttgart: Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker-Carus, C. (1980). Zustände des Bewußtseins. In W. Toman & W. F. Kugelmann (Hrsg.), Fernstudien im Medienverbund, Studieneinheit Allgemeine Psychologie. Erlangen-Nürnberg: (FIM) Fernstudium im Medienverbund.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker-Carus, C. (1981). Grundriß der Physiologischen Psychologie. Heidelberg: Quelle und Meyer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker-Carus, C., Heyden, T., & Ziegler, G. (1979). Psychophysiologische Methoden. Stuttgart: Enke.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker-Carus, & Heyden (1979). Streß-Wirkungen in Labor- und Realsituationen in Abhängigkeit von REM-Schlaf und Psychophysiologischer Aktivation. Zeitschrift für experimentelle und angewandte Psychologie, 26(1), 37–52.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Behrmann, M., Moscovitch, M., & Winocur, G. (1994). Intact visual imagery and impaired visual perception in a patient with visual agnosia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 1068–1087.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bennett, A. (1961). Sensory deprivation in aviation. In P. Solomon et al. (Hrsg.), Sensory deprivation (S. 161–173). Cambridge: Harvard.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berger, H. (1929). Über das Elektroencephalogramm des Menschen. Arch. Psychatr. Nerv. Krankenh., 87, 527–570.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berger, M. (Hrsg.). (1992). Handbuch des normalen und gesunden Schlafs. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berlucchi, G., Maffei, L., Moruzzi, G., & Strata, P. (1964). EEG and behavioral effects elicited by cooling of medulla and pons. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 102, 37–392.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bieri, P. (Hrsg.). (2007). Analytische Philosophie des Geistes (4. Aufl.). Weilheim: Beltz.

    Google Scholar 

  • Birbaumer, N., & Schmidt, R. F. (1999). Biologische Psychologie. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Blackmore, S. (1991). Lucid dreaming: Awake in your sleep? Sceptical Inquirer, 15, 362–370.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borbély, A. (1984). Das Geheimnis des Schlafs. München: DTV. Orig. 1984, Deutsche Verlagsanstalt GmbH Stuttgart

    Google Scholar 

  • Borbély, A. A., Baumann, F., Brandeis, D., Strauch, I., & Lehmann, D. (1981). Sleep deprivation: Effect on sleep stages and EEG power density in man. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 51, 483–493.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boucsein, W. (1992). Electrodermal activity. New York: Plenum Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Boulos, Z., & Terman, M. (1980). Food availability and daily biological rhythms. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 4, 119–131.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, M. (1959). Friend or traitor. Hypnosis in the service of religion. Intern J. of Clin. Exp. Hypnosis, 7, 205–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bremer, F. (1936). Nouvelle recherches sur le mécanisme du sommeil. Comptes Rendus de la Société de Biologie, 22, 460–464.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brendel, D. H., Reynolds 3rd, C. F., Jennings, J. R., Hoch, C. C., Monk, T. H., Berman, S. R., Hall, F. T., Buysse, D. J., & Kupfer, D. J. (1990). Sleep stage physiology, mood, and vigilance responses to total sleep deprivation in healthy 80-year-olds and 20-year-olds. Psychophysiology, 27, 677–685.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. (1954). The role of auditory localisation in attention and memory span. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47, 191–196.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and communication. New York: Pergamon.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S., & Goodman, C. C. (1947). Value and Need as Organizing Factors in Perception. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 42, 33–44. Buchholtz, C. (1982). Grundlagen der Verhaltensphysiologie. Braunschweig: Vieweg.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burney, C. (1952). Solitary confinement. New York: Coward-McCann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butter, C. M. (1968). Neurophysiology: The study of brain and behavior. Belmont: Brooks/Cole.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrd, R. (1938). Alone. New York: G. P. Putnam’s Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cave, K. R., & Wolfe, J. M. (1990). Modeling the role of parallel processing in visual search. Cognitive Psychology, 22, 225–271.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chalmers, D. J. (1995). Facing up to the problem of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 2, 200–219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chalmers, D. J. (1996). The Conscious Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chalmers, D. J. (Hrsg.). (2002). Philosophy of Mind: Classical and Contemporary Readings. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cherry, E. C. (1953). Some experiments on the recognition of speech with one and two ears. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 25, 975–979.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Churchland, P. M. (1985a). Reduction, qualia, and direct introspection of brain states. Journal of Philosophy, 82, 8–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Churchland, P. M. (1985b). Images of Science: Scientific Realism versus Constructive Empiricism. Chicago IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clarenbach, P., Klotz, U., Koella, W. P., & Rudolf, G. A. E. (1991). Schering Lexikon Schlafmedizin. München: MMV Medizinverlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coleman, R. M. (1986). Wide Awake at 3:00 A. M. New York: W. H. Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coles, M. G. H., Gratton, G., Bashore, T. R., Eriksen, C. W., & Donchin, E. (1985). A psychophysiological investigation of the continuous flow of human information processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11, 529–552.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Conway, A. R. A., Cowan, N., & Bunting, M. F. (2001). The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: The importance of working memory capacity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 331–335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 201–215.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Corbetta, M., Akbudak, E., Conturo, T. E., Snyder, A. Z., Ollinger, J. M., Drury, H. A., Linenweber, M. R., Petersen, S. E., Raichle, M. E., Van Essen, D. C., & Shulman, G. L. (1998). A common network of functional areas for attention and eye movements. Neuron, 21, 761–773.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Corbetta, M., Patel, G., & Shulman, G. L. (2008). The re-orienting system of the human brain: From environment to theory of mind. Neuron, 58, 306–324.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Corteen, R. S., & Wood, B. (1972). Autonomous responses to shock associated words in an unattended channel. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 94, 308–313.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cowen, E. L., & Beier, L. S. (1954). Threat-expectancy, word frequencies, and perceptual prerecognition hypotheses. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 49, 172–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cutzu, F., & Tsotsos, J. K. (2003). The selective tuning model of attention: psychophysical evidence for a suppressive annulus around an attended item. Vision Research, 43, 205–219.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R. (1999). How the brain creates the mind. Scientific American, 281(6), 74–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, H. (1964). Enhencement of the evoced cortical potentials in humans related to a task requiring a decision. Science, 145, 182–1183.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G. D. (1947). Cerebral responses to electrical stimulation of peripheral nerve in man. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiat., 10, 134–140.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dement, W. C. (1978). Some must watch while some must sleep. New York: W. W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. C. (1990). Quining qualia. In W. Lycan (Hrsg.), Mind and Cognition (S. 519–548). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. C. (1994). Philosophie des menschlichen Bewusstseins; übers. von Franz M. Wuketits. Hamburg: Hoffmann und Campe. Original 1991: Consciousness Explained, Boston: Little, Brown

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. C. (2003). Freedom Evolves (S. 519–548). New York: Viking.

    Google Scholar 

  • Derbyshire, A. J., Rempel, B., Forbes, A., & Lampert, E. F. (1936). The effect of anesthetics on action potentials in the cerebral cortex of cats. American Journal of Physiology, 116, 577–596.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deutsch, J. A., & Deutsch, D. (1963). Attention: Some theoretical considerations. Psychological Review, 70, 80–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dijk, D. J., Duffy, J. F., & Czeisler, C. A. (2000). Contribiution of circadian physiology and sleep homeostasis to age related changes in human sleep. Chronobiology international, 17(3), 285–311.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dörner, D. & Selg, H. (Hrsg.) (1985). Psychologie. Eine Einführung in ihre Grundlagen und Anwendungsfelder. Stuttgart: Kohlhammer. [7]

    Google Scholar 

  • Dodds, E. (1963). The greeks and the irrational. Berkley, CA: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dorsch, H. (Hrsg.), Psychologisches Wörterbuch. Bern: Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Driver, J., & Baylis, G. C. (1989). Movement and visual attention: The spotlight metaphor breaks down. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 448–456.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Driver, J., Davis, G., Ricciardelli, P., Kidd, P., Maxwell, E., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). Gaze perception triggers reflexive visuospatial orienting. Visual Cognition, 6, 509–540.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, J. (1984). Selective attention and the organization of visual information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 501–517.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, J., & Humphreys, G. W. (1989). Visual search and stimulus similarity. Psychological Review, 96, 433–458.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Edgar, D. M., & Dement, W. C. (1992). Evidence for opponent processes in sleep/wake regulation. Sleep Research, 20A, 2.

    Google Scholar 

  • Egly, R., Driver, J., & Rafal, R. D. (1994). Shifting visual attention between objects and locations: Evidence from normal and parietal lesion subjects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 161–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eimer, M., Van Velzen, J., & Driver, J. (2002). Crossmodal interactions between audition, touch, and vision in endogenous spatial attention: ERP evidence on preparatory states and sensory modulations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 254–271.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Empson, J. A. C., & Clarke, P. R. F. (1970). Rapid eye movements and remembering. Nature, 227, 287–288.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, S. (1972). The nature of anxiety with emphasis upun its relation to expectancy. In C. D. Spielberger (Hrsg.), Anxiety current trends in theory and research. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, C. W. (1995). The flanker task and response competition: A useful tool for investigating a variety of cognitive problems. Visual Cognition, 2, 101–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, C. W., Coles, M. G. H., Morris, L. R., & O’Hara, W. P. (1985). An electromyografic examination of response competition. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 23, 165–168. Eriksen, B. A. & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 143–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, C. W., & James, S. J. D. (1986). Visual attention within and around the field of focal attention: A zoom lens model. Perception & Psychophysics, 40, 225–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, C. (1984). Landscapes of the night. How and why we dream. New York: Viking.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenk, M. W., & Keane, M. T. (2010). Cognitive Psychology. Hove. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fahrenberg, J. (1972). Psychophysiologie. In W. Arnold, H. J. Eysenck & R. Meili (Hrsg.), Lexikon der Psychologie. Freiburg: Herder.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farthing, G. W. (1992). The psychology of consciousness. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fechner, G. T. (1860). Elemente der Psychophysik. Leipzig: Breitkopf und Hertel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fecteau, J. H., & Munoz, D. P. (2006). Salience, relevance, and firing: a priority map for target selection. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 382–390.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fehm, H. L., Kern, W., Pietrowsky, R., & Born, J. (1990). Schlaf und Harmonie. In K. Meyer-Ewert & H. Schulz (Hrsg.), Schlaf und Schlafstörungen. Berlin, Heidelberg, New York, Tokyo: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Field, M. (1960). Search for security: an ethnopsychiatric study of rural Ghana. Evanstan: Northstern University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flechtner, H. S. (1968). Grundbegriffe der Kybernetik. Stuttgart: Wiss. Verlagsgesellschaft.

    Google Scholar 

  • Floderus-Myrhed, B., Pedersen, N., & Rasmussen, I. (1980). Assessment of heritability for personality, based on a short form of Eysenck Personality Inventory: A study of 12.898 twin pairs. Behavior Genetics, 10, 507–520.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. (1983). The Modularity of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 1030–1044.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Foulkes, W. D. (1969). Die Psychologie des Schlafes. Frankfurt/M: Fischer. Orig. 1966, The Psychology of sleep. New York: Scribner’s

    Google Scholar 

  • Found, A., & Müller, H. (1996). Searching for unknown feature targets on more than one dimension: Investigating a „dimension-weighting“ account. Perception & Psychophysics, 58, 88–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, E. (1995). Negative priming from ignored distractors in visual selection: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 145–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • French, J. D. (1957). The reticular formation. Scientific American, 5, 191–198. Auch in: N. Chalmers, R. Crawley & S. P. R. Rose (Hrsg.) (1971), The biological bases of behavior. London: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S. (1940/1972). Abriss der Psychoanalyse. (Gesammelte Werke Bd. 17), Frankfurt/M.: Fischer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S. (1956). Zur Psychopathologie des Alltagslebens. Frankfurt/M: Fischer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S. (1900/1973). Die Traumdeutung (Gesammelte Werke, Bd. 2/3). Frankfurt/M.: Fischer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friesen, C. K., & Kingstone, A. (1998). The eyes have it! Reflexive orienting triggered by nonpredictive gaze. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5, 490–495.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuster, J. M. (1958). Effects of stimulation of brain stem on tachistoscopic perception. Science,, 127, 150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, W. (1953). The boat. Boston: Riverside Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gill, M., & Brennan, M. (1959). Hypnosis and related states: Psychoanalytic studies in regression. New York: International University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gusky, R. (1989). Wahrnehmung. Stuttgart: Kohlhammer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guttman, G. (1972). Einführung in die Neuropsychologie. Bern u. a.: Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. (1949). The organization of behavior: a neuropsychological theory. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heinze, H. J., Luck, S. J., Münte, T. F., Gös, A., Mangun, G. R., & Hillyard, S. A. (1994). Attention to adjacent and separate positions in space:  An electrophysiological analysis. Perception & Psychophysics, 56, 42–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herbart, J. F. (1816). Lehrbuch zur Psychologie. Königsberg: Saur.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hernandez-Peón, R., Scherrer, H., & Jouvet, M. (1956). Modification of electric activity in cochlear nucleus during attention in unanesthetized cats. Science, 123, 331–332.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heron, W. (1957). The pathology of boredom. Scientific American, 196, 52–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hillyard, S. A., & Anllo-Vento, L. (1998). Event-related brain potentials in the study of visual selective attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 95, 781–787.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hillyard, S. A., Hink, R. F., Schwent, V. L., & Picton, T. W. (1973). Electrical signs of selective attention in the human brain. Science, 182, 177–179.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, J. A. (1988). The dreaming brain. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, J. A. (1990). Schlaf, Gehirnaktivität im Ruhezustand. Heidelberg: Spektrum. Orig. 1989, Sleep. New York: Freeman

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, J. A. (1994). The chemistry of consciousness states. New York: Little, Brown.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, J. A., & McCarley, R. W. (1977). The brain as a dream state generator: An activation-synthesis hypothesis of the dream process. American Journal of Psychiatry, 134, 1225–1348.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hofstätter, P. R. (1964). Psychologie. Frankfurt: Fischerbücherei.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hofstätter, P. R. (1966/1972). Psychologie. Frankfurt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hokanson, J. E. (1969). The psychological basis of motivation. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horne, J. A. (1976). Recovery sleep following different visual conditions during total sleep deprivation in man. Biological Psychology, 4, 107–118.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horne, J. A. (1983). Mammalian sleep function with particular reference to man. In A. R. Mayes (Hrsg.), Sleep mechanisms and functions in humans and animals (S. 262–312). Wokinham: Van Nostrand Reinhold.

    Google Scholar 

  • Husain, M., & Rorden, C. (2003). Non-spatially lateralized mechanisms in hemispatial neglect. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 26–36. PMID:12511859.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, F. (1998). Postscript on qualia. In F. Jackson (Hrsg.), Mind, Method and Conditionals. London: Routledge.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Jaspers, K. (1913 / 1923). Allgemeine Psychopathologie. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jouvet, M. (1961). Telencephalic and rhombencephalic sleep in the cat. In G. E. W. Wolstenhome & C. M. O’Connor (Hrsg.), The nature of sleep. Boston: Little Brown.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jouvet, M. (1967). Neurophysiology of the states of sleep. Physiol. Rev., 47, 117–177.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jovanovic, N. (1971). Normal sleep in man. Stuttgart: Hippocrates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Juola, J. F., Bowhuis, D. G., Cooper, E. E., & Warner, C. B. (1991). Control of attention around the fovea. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 315–330.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karni, A., & Sagi, D. (1994). Dependence on REM sleep for overnight improvement of perceptual skills. Science, 265, 679–682.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2003). Working memory capacity and the control of attention: the contribution of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 47–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kant, I. (1781/1787). Kritik an der reinen Vernunft. (Hrsg.: K. Kehrbach). Leipzig: Reclam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karadžić, V. T. (1973). Physiological changes resulting from total sleep deprivation. 1st European Congress on Sleep Research, Basel, 1972. Basel: Karger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keidel, W. D. (1973). Kurzgefaßtes Lehrbuch der Physiologie. Stuttgart: Thieme.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kesner, R. P. (1977). A neural system approach to the study of memory storage and retrieval. In Drucker-Colin (Hrsg.), Neurobiology of sleep & memory. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, K. E., Wegmann, H. M., & Hunt, B. M. (1972). Desynchronization of body temperature and performance circadian rhythm as a result of outgoing and home-going transmeridian flights. Aerospace Medicine, 43, 119–132.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kleitman, N. (1963). Sleep and Wakefulness. Chicago: University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klimowa, V. I. (1958). The properties of the components of some orientation reactions. In The orientation reaction and orienting-investigation of activity. Moscow: Academy of Pedagogical Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knauth, P., & Rutenfranz, J. (1992). Störungen bei Verschiebungen des Schlaf-Wach-Zyklus. In M. Berger (Hrsg.), Handbuch des normalen und gestörten Schlafs. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koella, W. (1973). Physiologie des Schlafes. Stuttgart: Kohlhammer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kornhuber, H. H., & Deecke, L. (1965). Hirnpotentialänderungen bei Willkürbewegungen und passiven Bewegungen des Menschen: Bereitschaftspotential und reafferente Potentiale. Pflüg. Arch., 284, 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • La Barre, W. (1962). They shall take up serpents. Minneapolis: University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • La Barre, W. (1964). The peyote cult. Hamden: Shoe String Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, D. (1983). Spatial extent of attention to letters and words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 371–379.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, S. (1986). Lucid dreaming. New York: Ballantine.

    Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, D., & Brown, V. (1989). A model of perceptual classification in children and adults. Psychological Review, 96, 125–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lamb, M. R., Pond, H. M., & Zahir, G. (2000). Contributions of automatic and controlled processes to the analysis of hierarchical structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 234–245.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lambert, W. W., Solomon, R. L., & Watson, P. D. (1949). Reinforcement and extinction as factors in size estimation. Journal of experimental Psychology, 39, 637–641.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lavie, N. (1995). Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 451–468.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lavie, N., Hirst, A., de Fockert, J. W., & Viding, E. (2004). Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 339–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Legewie, H., & Ehlers, W. (1972). Knauers moderne Psychologie (1. Aufl.). München: Drömer Knaur.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leopold, D. A., & Logothetis, N. K. (1996). Activity changes in early visual cortex reflect monkeys‘ percepts during binocular rivalry. Nature, 379, 549–553.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levine, J. (2001). Purple Haze: The Puzzle of Conscious Experience. Cambridge: The MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, J. L. (1970). Semantic processing of unattended messages using dichotic listening. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 85, 225–228.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lexikon der Biologie. Heidelberg: Spektrum Akademischer Verlag

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindsley, D. B. (1958). The reticular system and perceptual discrimination. In H. H. Jasper (Hrsg.), Reticular formation of the brain (S. 513–534). Boston: Little Brown.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindsley, D. B. (1961). Common factors in sensory deprivation, sensory distortion, and sensory overload. In P. Salomon (Hrsg.), Sensory deprivation (S. 174–194). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindsley, D. B., Bowden, J. W., & Magoun, H. W. (1949). Effect upon the EEG of acute injury to the brain stem activating system. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 1, 475–486.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Logothetis, N. K. (2000). Das Sehen – ein Fenster zum Bewusstsein. Spektrum der Wissenschaft, 1, 63–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loomis, A. L., Harvey, E. N., & Hobart, G. H. (1937). Cerebral status during sleep as studied by human brain potentials. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 21, 127–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luck, S. J., Chelazzi, L., Hillyard, S. A., & Desimone, R. (1997). Neural mechanisms of spatial selective attention in areas V1, V2, and V4 of macaque visual cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 77, 24–42.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ludwig, A. (1965). Witchcraft today. Disorders of the nervous system, 26, 288–291.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ludwig, A. (1969). Altered states of consciousness. In C. T. Tart (Hrsg.), Altered states of consciousness (S. 9–22). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ludwig, A., & Levine, J. (1966). Clinical effects of psychodelic agents. Clinical Medicine, 73, 21–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lumer, E. D., Friston, K. J., & Rees, G. (1998). Neural correlates of perceptual rivalry in the human brain. Science, 280, 1930–1934.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lydic, R., McCarley, R. W., & Hobson, J. A. (1983). The time-course of dorsal raphé discharche. PGO waves, and muscle tone averaged across multiple sleep cycles. Brain Research, 274, 365–370.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald 3rd, A. W., Cohen, J. D., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2000). Dissociating the role of dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulated cortex in cognitive control. Science, 288, 1835–1838.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Magni, F., Moruzzi, G., Rossi, G. F., & Zanchetti, A. (1957). EEG arousal following inactivation of the lower brain stem by selective injection of barbiturate into the vertebral circulation. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 95, 33–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mangun, G. R., & Hillyard Luck, S. A. S. (1993). Electrocortical substrates of visual selective attention. In D. E. Meyer & S. Kornblum (Hrsg.), Attention and Performance XIV: Synergies in experimental psychology, artificial intelligence, and cognitive neuroscience (S. 219–243). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • May, C. P., Kane, M. J., & Hasher, L. (1995). Determinants of negative priming. Psychological Bulletin, 118, 35–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McGinnies, E. (1949). Emotionality and perceptual defense. Psychological Review, 56, 244–251.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McGlinchey-Berroth, R., Milberg, W. P., Verfaellie, M., Alexander, M., & Kilduff, P. T. (1993). Semantic processing in the neglected visual field: Evidence from a lexical decision task. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 10, 79–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGrath, M. J., & Cohen, D. G. (1978). REM sleep fascilitation of adaptive waking behavior; A review of the literature. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 24–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Michel, S., Geusz, M. E., Zaritsky, J. J., & Block, G. D. (1993). Circadian rhythm in membrane conductance expressed in isolated neurons. Science, 259, 239–241.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mistlberger, R. E. (1993). Circadian properties of anticipatory activity to restricted water access in suprachiasmatic-ablated hamsters. American Journal of Physiology, 263, 22–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore, R. Y., & Eichler, V. B. (1972). Loss of a circadian adrenal corticosterone rhythm following suprachiasmatic lesions in the rat. Brain Research, 42, 201–206.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, R. Y. (1999). Circadian timing. In M. J. Zigmund et al. (Hrsg.), Fundamental Neuroscience. San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morawetz, C., Holz, P., Baudewig, J., Treue, S., & Dechent, P. (2007). Split of attentional resources in human visual cortex. Visual Neuroscience, 24, 817–826.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moray, N. (1959). Attention in dichotic listening: Affective cues and the influence of instruction. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 11, 56–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moruzzi, G., & Magoun, H. W. (1949). Brain stem reticular formation and activation of the EEG. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 1, 455–473.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mullaney, D. J., Johnson, L. C., Naitoh, P., Friedman, J. K., & Globus, G. G. (1977). Sleep during and after gradual sleep reduction. Psychophysiology, 14, 237–244.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Müller, H. J., Heller, D., & Ziegler, J. (1995). Visual search for singleton feature targets within and across feature dimensions. Perception & Psychophysics, 57, 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Müller, H. J., Reimann, B., & Krummenacher, J. (2003a). Visual search for singleton feature targets across dimensions: Stimulus- and expectancy-driven effects in dimensional weighting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 29, 1021–1035.

    Google Scholar 

  • Müller, M. M., & Hübner, R. (2002). Can the spotlight of attention be shaped like a doughnut? Evidence from steady-state visual evoked potentials. Psychological Science, 13, 119–124.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Müller, N. G., Bartelt, O. A., Donner, T. H., Villringer, A., & Brandt, S. A. (2003b). A physiological correlate of the „zoom lens“ of visual attention. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 3561–3565.

    Google Scholar 

  • Müller, N. G., Mollenhauer, M., Rösler, A., & Kleinschmidt, A. (2005). The attentional field has a Mexican hat distribution. Vision Research, 45, 1129–1137.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Müller, T. (1996). Wieviel Schlaf braucht der Mensch? Auswirkung chronischer Schlafrestriktion. Münster: Lit.

    Google Scholar 

  • Müller, T., & Paterok, B. (1999). Schlaftraining. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, J. (1964). Psychotherapeutic aspects of shamanism on St. Lawrence Island, Alaska. In A. Kiev (Hrsg.), Magic faith and healing (S. 53–83). New York: Free of Glencoe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Myers, D. G. (19861/19985). Psychology. New York: Worth Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Myers, D. G. (2008, 2014). Psychologie. Heidelberg: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagel, T. (1974). What is it like to be a bat? Philosophical Review, 83, 435–456.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neill, W. T., Valdes, L. A., & Terry, K. M. (1995). Selective attention and the inhibitory control of cognition. In F. N. Dempster & C. J. Brainerd (Hrsg.), Interference and inhibition in cognition (S. 207–261). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, P. (2001). Grundlagen. Markt- und Werbepsychologie, Bd. 1. Gräfelfing: Fachverl. Wirtschaftspsychologie.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Fredrickson, B. L., Loftus, G. R., & Lutz, C. (2014). Atkinson & Hilgard’s Introduction to Psychology. Hampshire: Cengage Learning EMEA.

    Google Scholar 

  • OʼCraven, K., Downing, P., & Kanwisher, N. (1999). fMRI evidence for objects as the units of attentional selection. Nature, 401, 584–587.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A. (1976). Memory and Attention: An Introduction to Human Information Processing. New York: Wiley. deutsch 1973, übersetzt nach 1. Aufl., Aufmerksamkeit und Gedächtnis. Eine Einführung in die menschliche Informationsverarbeitung. Weinheim: Beltz

    Google Scholar 

  • Orme-Johnson, N., & Farrow, J. T. (1977). Scientific research on the transcendental Meditation program. Bd. 1. Fairfield, IOWA: Maharishi European University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oswald, I. (1969). Human brain protein, drugs, and dreams. Nature, 223, 893–897.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Palumbo, S. R. (1978). Dreaming and memory: A new information-processing model. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pearlman, C., & Greenberg, R. (1973). Posttrial REM sleep: A critical period for consolidation of shuttlebox avoidance. Animal Learning and Behaviour, 1, 49–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piéron, H. (1913). Le problème physiologique du sommeil. Paris: Masson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinel, P. J. (1997). Biopsychologie – eine Einführung. Heidelberg: Spektrum Akademischer Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinel, J. P. J. (2000). Biopsychology (4. Aufl.). Needham Heights: Allyn and Bacon. deutsch: Pinel, P. J. (1997). Biopsychologie – eine Einführung. Heidelberg, Spektrum Akademischer Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Popper, K. R. & Eccles, J. C. (19771/1982/1985). Das Ich und sein Gehirn. München: Piper.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R., & Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 160–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., Walker, J. A., Friedrich, F. J., & Rafal, R. D. (1984). Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention. Journal of Neuroscience, 4, 1863–1874.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, H. (1975). Philosophy and our mental life. In H. Putnam (Hrsg.), Mind Language and Reality: Philosophical Papers Bd. 2 Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Qualtrochi, J. J., Mamelak, A. N., Madison, R. D., Macklis, J. D., & Hobson, J. A. (1989). Mapping neuronal inputs to REM sleep introduction sites with carbachol-fluorescent microspheres. Science, 245, 984–986.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ralph, M. R., Foster, T. G., Davis, F. C., & Menaker, M. (1990). Transplantic suprachiasmatic nucleus determines circadian period. Science, 247, 975–978.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ramachandran, V. S., Levi, L., Stone, L., & Rogers-Ramachandran, D. (1996). Illusions of body image: What they reveal about human nature. In R. R. Llinas & P. S. Churchland (Hrsg.), The mind-brain continuum: Sensory processes (S. 29–60). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rechtschaffen, A., & Kales, A. (1968). A manual of standardized terminology, techniques and scoring systems for sleep stages of human subjects. Washington, DC: U. S. Government Printing Office.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richter, C. P. (1967). Sleep and activity: Their relation to the 24-hour clock. Proceedings of the Association for Research on Nervous and Mental disorders, 45, 8–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rohracher, H. (1971). Leib-Seele-Problem. In W. Arnold et al. (Hrsg.), Lexikon der Psychologie. Freiburg: Herder.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubinstein, S. L. (1971). Grundlagen der allgemeinen Psychologie. Berlin: VEB Volk und Wissen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruch, F. L., & Zimbardo, P. G. (1974). Lehrbuch der Psychologie. Berlin: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Rusak, B., & Zucker, I. (1979). Neural regulation of circadian rhythms. Psychological Reviews, 59, 449–528.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rusak, B., Robertson, H. A., Wisden, W., & Hunt, S. (1990). Light pulses that shift rhythms induce gene expression in the suprachiasmatic nucleus. Science, 248, 1237–1240.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salamy, J. (1970). Instrumental responding to internal cues associated with REM sleep. Psychonomic Science, 18, 342–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sargant, W. (1957). Battle for the mind. Garden City, New York: Doubleday.

    Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. (1980). Minds, Brains and Programs. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheinberg, D. L., & Logothetis, N. K. (1997). The role of temporal cortical areas in perceptual organization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 94, 3408–3413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simons, D. J., & Chabris, C. F. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28, 1059–1074.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sokolov, E. N. (1960). Neuronal models and the orienting reflex. In M. A. Brazier (Hrsg.), The central nervous system and behaviour. New York: Josiah Macy.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperling, G. A. (1967). Successive Approximations to a Model for Short-term Memory. Acta Psychologica, 27, 285–292.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spoerri, T. (1965). Kompendium der Psychiatrie. Frankfurt/M: Akademische Verlagsanstalt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Squier, L. H. & Domhoff, G. W. (1998). The presentation of dreaming and dreams in introductory psychology textbooks: A critical examination. Unveröffentlichtes Manuskript.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steinbuch, K. (1961). Automat und Mensch. Heidelberg: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Steiner, R. (1904/1972). Wie erlangt man Erkenntnisse höherer Welten? Dornach/Schweiz: Gesamtausgabe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stephan, F. K., & Zucker, I. (1972). Circadian rhythms in drinking behaviour and locomotor activity of rats are eliminated by hypothalamic lesions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science (USA), 60, 1583–1586.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tart, C. T. (1969). Altered states of consciousness. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tart, C. T. (1972). Scientific foundation for the study of altered states of consciousness. Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 3, 93–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Theeuwes, J. (1991). Exogenous and endogenous control of attention: The effect of visual onsets and offsets. Perception & Psychophysics, 49, 83–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Theeuwes, J. (1992). Perceptual selectivity for color and form. Perception & Psychophysics, 51, 599–606.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomson, R. F. (1975). Introduction to physiological psychology. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thigpen, C. H., & Cleckley, H. A. (1954). Case of multiple personality. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 49(1), 135–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thigpen, C. H., & Cleckley, H. A. (1957). The three faces of eve. New York: McGraw-Hill. Tinbergen, N. (1956).Instinktlehre. Hamburg: Parey. Original: 1951. The Study of Instinct. London: Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen, N. (1966). Tiere und ihr Verhalten. Life, Wunder der Natur. Amsterdam, Nederland: Time-Life International.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tipper, S. P. (1985). The negative priming effect: Inhibitory priming by ignored objects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 37A, 571–590.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tipper, S. P., & Driver, J. (1988). Negative priming between pictures and words in a selective attention task: evidence for semantic processing of ignored stimuli. Memory & Cognition, 16, 64–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tootell, R. B., Hadjikhani, N., Hall, E. K., Marrett, S., Vanduffel, W., Vaughan, J. T., & Dale, A. M. (1998). The retinotopy of visual spatial attention. Neuron, 21, 1409–1422.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A. M. (1960). Contextual cues in selective listening. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12, 242–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A. M. (1986). Features and objects in visual processing. Scientific American, 254, 114–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97–136.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Trulson, M. E., & Jacobs, B. L. (1979). Raphe unit activity in freely moving cats: Correlation with level of behavioural arousal. Brain research, 163, 135–350.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vertes, R. P. (1983). Brainstem control of the events of REM sleep. Progress in Neurobiology, 22, 241–288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walter, W. G., Cooper, R., Aldridge, V. J., McCallum, W. C., & Winter, A. L. (1964). Contingent negative variation: an electric sign of sensorimotor association and expectancy. Nature, 203, 380–384.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Webb, W. B., & Agnew Jr., H. W. (1967). Sleep cycling within the twenty-four hour period. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 74, 167–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Webb, W. B., & Bonnet, M. H. (1978). The sleep of „morning“ and „evening“ types. Biological Psychology, 7, 29–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Von Weizsäcker, C. F. (1988, 1991). Bewusstseinswandel. München: Hanser.

    Google Scholar 

  • Westheide, W., & Rieger, R. (2006). Spezielle Zoologie. Teil 1: Einzeller und Wirbellose Tiere. Heidelberg: Spektrum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wever, R. (1980). Die Tagesperiodik des Menschen. Grundlagen und Probleme. Betriebsärztliches, 1, 1–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wever, R. (1979). The Circadian System of Man. New York: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wever, R. (1989). Light effects on human circadian rhythms: a review of recent Andechs experiments.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitman, R. M., Pierce, C. M., & Maas, J. W. (1962). The dreams of the experimental subject. Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease, 134(5), 431–439.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wickelgren, W. A. (1968). Effect of state of arousal on click evoked responses in cats. Journal of Neurophysiology, 31, 757–768.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, G. (1958). Hypnosis in perspective. In L. Le Cron (Hrsg.), Experimental hypnosis (S. 4–21). New York: Mac Millan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, J. M. (1994). Guided Search 2.0: A Revised Model of Visual Search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 202–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, J. M. (1998). Visual Search. In H. Pashler (Hrsg.), Attention (S. 13–68). London: University College London Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wundt, W. (1874). 1893). Grundzüge einer physiologischen Psychologie. Leipzig: Engelmann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimbardo, & Ruch (1971). Psychology and Life (8. Aufl.). Brief. Copyright © 1971,1967 by Scott, Foresman and Company, Glenview, Illinois 60025, USA (deutsch: Zimbardo & Ruch (1978) Lehrbuch der Psychologie (3. Auflage). Berlin: Springer.)

    Google Scholar 

  • Zulley, J. (1979). Der Einfluß von Zeitgebern auf den Schlaf des Menschen. Frankfurt: Rita G. Fische. Dissertation Tübingen

    Google Scholar 

  • Zulley, J. (1993). Schlafen und Wachen als biologischer Rhythmus. Regensburg: Roderer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zulley, J. (2002/ 2000). Zirkadiane Rhythmen und Schlaf beim Menschen. In: H. Schulz (Hrsg.), Kompendium Schlafmedizin. Landsberg: ecomed.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Christian Becker-Carus .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 Springer-Verlag GmbH Deutschland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Becker-Carus, C., Wendt, M. (2017). Aufmerksamkeit und Bewusstsein. In: Allgemeine Psychologie. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-53006-1_5

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-53006-1_5

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-662-53005-4

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-662-53006-1

  • eBook Packages: Psychology (German Language)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics