Skip to main content

Subjective Rationality and Cultural Diversity

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Methodological Cognitivism
  • 844 Accesses

Abstract

If social theory has to be policy-relevant, it has to use a not too unrealistic “model of man”, even though any model represents a drastic simplification of the real world. In practice, however, even simple beliefs or the most familiar types of behavior that we observe in everyday life can only be explained with difficulty by the two dominant models to which the familiar labels of Homo sociologicus and Homo oeconomicus are attached respectively. Redressing this situation may be one of the most challenging problems facing contemporary theory.

The present chapter is a modified version of Boudon, R., & Viale, R., (2000). Reason, cognition and society. published in Mind & Society, 1,vol. 1.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Berlin, B. O., & Kay, P. D. (1969). Basic color terms. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boudon, R. (1981). The logic of social action. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Choi, S., & Bowerman, M. (1991). Learning to express motion events in English and Korean: The influence of language-specific lexicalization patterns. Cognition, 41, 83–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Choi, I., Nisbett, R. E., & Smith, E. E. (1997). Culture, categorization and inductive reasoning. Cognition, 65, 15–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M. (1977). An ethnographic psychology of cognition. In P. Johnson-Laird & P. C. Wason (Eds.), Thinking. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Donovan, A., Laudan, L., & Laudan, R. (Eds.). (1988). Scrutinizing science. Empirical studies of scientific change. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durkheim, E. (1897). Suicide, a study in sociology. New York: The Free Press. 1962.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durkheim, E. (1912). Le formes élementaires de la vie religieuse. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France. 1979.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elster, J. (1985). Making sense of Marx. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans-Pritchard, E. E. (1937). Witchcraft, oracles and magic among the azande. Oxford: Clarendon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gladwin, T. (1970). East is a Big bird. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenfield, P.M. & Childs, C. (1974). Weaving, color terms and patterns representation: cultural influences and cognitive development among Zinacantecos of southern Mexico, in J. Dawson & W. Lonner (Eds.) Reading in cross-cultural psychology. Proceedings of the first international conference of the international association for cross-cultural psychology (Hong Kong, University of Hong Kong Press)

    Google Scholar 

  • Huber, J., Payne, J. W., & Pluto, C. (1982). Adding asymmetrical dominated alternatives: Violations of regularity and similarity hypothesis. Journal of Consumer Research, 9, 90–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979b). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47, 263–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, S. C. (1994). Vision, shape and linguistic description: Tzeltal body part terminology and object description. Linguistics, 32, 791–885.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lopez, A., Atran, S., Coley, J. D., Medin, D. L., & Smith, E. E. (1997). The tree of life: Universal and cultural features of folkbiological taxonomies and inductions. Cognitive Psychology, 32(3), 251–295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malinowski, B. (1954). Magic, science and religion. New York: Doubleday.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marx, K. (1867). Das Kapital. Hamburg/New York: O. Meissner/L.W. Schmidt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Medin, D. L., Lynch, E., Coley, J. D., & Atran, S. (1997). Categorization and reasoning among tree experts: Do all roads lead to Rome? Cognitive Psychology, 32(1), 49–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Myrvold, W. C. (1996). Bayesianism and diverse evidence: A reply to Andrew Wayne. Philosophy of Science, 63, 661–665.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olson M. (1965). The logic of collective action. Cambridge Mass: Harvard University Press. (Trad. it., 1983), La logica dell’azione collettiva, Milano, Feltrinelli.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osherson, D., Smith, E., Wilkie, O., López, A., & Shafir, E. (1990). Category-based induction. Psychological Review, 97(2), 185–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, E. (1972). The structure of color space in naming and memory for Two languages. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 337–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shafir, E. (1993). Choosing versus rejecting: why some options are both better and worse than others. Memory & Cognition, 21, 546–556.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shafir, E., Simonson, I., & Tversky, A. (1993). Reason-based choice. Cognition, 49, 11–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shweder, R. A. (1977). Likeness and likelihood in everyday thought: Magical thinking and everyday judgments about personality. In P. N. Johnson-Laird & P. C. Wason (Eds.), Thinking. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. (1982). Models of bounded rationality. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simonson, I., & Tversky, A. (1992). Choice in context: Tradeoff contrast and extremeness aversion. Journal of Marketing Research, 29, 281–295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tambiah, S. J. (1973). Form and meaning of magical acts: A point of view. In R. Horton & R. Finnegan (Eds.), Models of thought. London: Faber and Faber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5, 207–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Viale, R., & Osherson, D. (2000). The diversity principle and the little scientist hypothesis. Foundations of Science, 5(2), 239–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • von Neumann, J., & Morgenstern, O. (1947). Theory of games and economic behavior. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weber, M. (1922). Wirtschaft und Gesellschaft. Tubingen: Mohr.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Riccardo Viale .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2012 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Viale, R. (2012). Subjective Rationality and Cultural Diversity. In: Methodological Cognitivism. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-24743-9_12

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics