Skip to main content

Choosing to Learn: Evidence Evaluation for Active Learning and Teaching in Early Childhood

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Active Learning from Infancy to Childhood

Abstract

Choosing to take certain actions has direct consequences for learning. Do children tend to choose actions that support learning? We present research suggesting that before reaching kindergarten, children demonstrate proficiency in active learning that cannot be accounted for by simple heuristics for decision-making. Instead, children’s choices reveal a sophisticated ability to evaluate evidence (computing information gained by particular actions); potential evidence is compared to other possible rewards and costs associated with actions. This early emerging ability to select evidence underpins informal teaching of, and reasoning about, others. Specifically, we discuss recent empirical work demonstrating that preschool-aged children are able to evaluate potential evidence to support their own learning, another’s learning, and that they can even evaluate a teacher’s evidential selection for a third party’s learning.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 99.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 139.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Bass, I., Bonawitz, E., & Gweon, H. (2017). Didn’t know, or didn’t show? Preschoolers consider epistemic state and degree of omission when evaluating teachers. In G. Gunzelmann, A. Howes, T. Tenbrink, & E. Davelaar (Eds.), Proceedings of the 39th annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 105–110). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bass, I., Bonawitz, E., Shafto, P., Hanson, M., Ramarajan, D., Gopnik, A., & Wellman, H. (in review). Children’s developing theory of mind and pedagogical evidence selection.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berlyne, D. E. (1978). Curiosity and learning. Motivation and Emotion, 2(2), 97–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bonawitz, E., Denison, S., Gopnik, A., & Griffiths, T. L. (2014). Win-Stay, Lose-Sample: A simple sequential algorithm for approximating Bayesian inference. Cognitive Psychology, 74, 35–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bonawitz, E., Denison, S., Griffiths, T. L., & Gopnik, A. (2014). Probabilistic models, learning algorithms, and response variability: Sampling in cognitive development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(10), 497–500.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bonawitz, E., Shafto, P., Gweon, H., Goodman, N. D., Spelke, E., & Schulz, L. (2011). The double-edged sword of pedagogy: Instruction limits spontaneous exploration and discovery. Cognition, 120(3), 322–330.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bonawitz, E., Ullman, T., Gopnik, A., & Tenenbaum, J. (2012, November). Sticking to the Evidence? A computational and behavioral case study of micro-theory change in the domain of magnetism. In 2012 IEEE international conference on development and learning and epigenetic robotics (ICDL) (pp. 1–6). IEEE.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bonawitz, E. B., Fischer, A., & Schulz, L. (2012). Teaching 3.5-year-olds to revise their beliefs given ambiguous evidence. Journal of Cognition and Development, 13(2), 266–280. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2011.577701

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bonawitz, E. B., Ullman, T., Bridgers, S., Gopnik, A., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (in revision). Chicken-and-egg: A computational, historical, and behavioral case study of micro-theory change in the domain of magnetism.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bonawitz, E. B., van Schijndel, T. J., Friel, D., & Schulz, L. (2012). Children balance theories and evidence in exploration, explanation, and learning. Cognitive Psychology, 64(4), 215–234. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2011.12.002

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Callanan, M. A., & Oakes, L. M. (1992). Preschoolers’ questions and parents’ explanations: Causal thinking in everyday activity. Cognitive Development, 7, 213–233. https://doi.org/10.1016/0885-2014(92)90012-G

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chouinard, M. M. (2007). Children’s questions: A mechanism for cognitive development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 72, 7–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, C., Goodman, N. D., & Schulz, L. E. (2011). Where science starts: Spontaneous experiments in preschoolers’ exploratory play. Cognition, 120(3), 341–349.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). Flow and the psychology of discovery and invention. New York, NY: Harper Collins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Day, H. I., & Langevin, R. (1969). Curiosity and intelligence: Two necessary conditions for a high level of creativity. The Journal of Special Education, 3(3), 263–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denison, S., Bonawitz, E., Gopnik, A., & Griffiths, T. L. (2013). Rational variability in children’s causal inferences: The sampling hypothesis. Cognition, 126(2), 285–300.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, K., & Klahr, D. (1989). Developmental differences in scientific discovery processes. In D. Klahr & K. Kotovsky (Eds.), The 21st Carnegie-Mellon symposium on cognition: Complex information processing: The impact of Herbert A. Simon. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gonzalez, A., Shafto, P., Bonawtiz, E. B., & Gopnik, A. (2012). Is that your final answer? The effects of neutral queries on children’s choices. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 34(34).

    Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A. (2000). Explanation as orgasm and the drive for causal knowledge: The function, evolution, and phenomenology of the theory formation system. In F. Keil & R. Wilson (Eds.), Cognition and explanation (pp. 299–323). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A. (2009). The philosophical baby: What children's minds tell us about truth, love and the meaning of life. New York, NY: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gweon, H., & Asaba, M. (2017). Order matters: Children’s evaluation of under-informative teachers depends on context. Child Development. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12825

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gweon, H., Pelton, H., Konopka, J. A., & Schulz, L. E. (2014). Sins of omission: Children selectively explore when agents fail to tell the whole truth. Cognition, 132(3), 335–341. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.04.013

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gweon, H., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Schulz, L. E. (2010). Infants consider both the sample and the sampling process in inductive generalization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(20), 9066–9071.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jaswal, V. K., & Neely, L. A. (2006). Adults don't always know best preschoolers use past reliability over age when learning new words. Psychological Science, 17(9), 757–758. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01778.x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jirout, J., & Klahr, D. (2012). Children’s scientific curiosity: In search of an operational definition of an elusive concept. Developmental Review, 32(2), 125–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jowett, B. (Ed.). (1941). Plato’s the republic. New York, NY: The Modern Library.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kidd, C., Piantadosi, S. T., & Aslin, R. N. (2012). The Goldilocks effect: Human infants allocate attention to visual sequences that are neither too simple nor too complex. PLoS One, 7(5), e36399. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0036399

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kidd, C., Piantadosi, S. T., & Aslin, R. N. (2014). The Goldilocks effect in infant auditory attention. Child Development, 85(5), 1795–1804. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12263

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Knudsen, B., & Liszkowski, U. (2012). One-year-olds warn others about negative action outcomes. Journal of Cognition and Development, 14(3), 424–436. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2012.689387

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koenig, M. A., Clément, F., & Harris, P. L. (2004). Trust in testimony: Children’s use of true and false statements. Psychological Science, 15(10), 694–698. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00742.x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koerber, S., Sodian, B., Thoermer, C., & Nett, U. (2005). Scientific reasoning in young children: Preschoolers’ ability to evaluate covariation evidence. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 64(3), 141–152. https://doi.org/10.1024/1421-0185.64.3.141

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D. (1989). Children and adults as intuitive scientists. Psychological Review, 96(4), 674–689.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D., Amsel, E., & O'Laughlin, M. (1988). The development of scientific thinking skills. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D., & Phelps, E. (1982). The development of problem-solving strategies. In H. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 17, pp. 1–44). New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lapidow, E., & Bonawitz, E. (in review). Rational action: Ambiguity, expectation, and information gain influence preschooler’s choices in exploration.

    Google Scholar 

  • Legare, C. H., Gelman, S. A., & Wellman, H. M. (2010). Inconsistency with prior knowledge triggers children’s causal explanatory reasoning. Child Development, 81(3), 929–944. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01443.x

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Leonard, N. H., & Harvey, M. (2007). The trait of curiosity as a predictor of emotional intelligence. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 37(8), 1914–1929.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Twelve-month-olds communicate helpfully and appropriately for knowledgeable and ignorant partners. Cognition, 108(3), 732–739. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.06.013

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein, G. (1994). The psychology of curiosity: A review and reinterpretation. Psychological Bulletin, 116(1), 75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markant, D. B., Ruggeri, A., Gureckis, T. M., & Xu, F. (2016). Enhanced memory as a common effect of active learning. Mind, Brain, and Education, 10(3), 142–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pasquini, E. S., Corriveau, K. H., Koenig, M., & Harris, P. L. (2007). Preschoolers monitor the relative accuracy of informants. Developmental Psychology, 43(5), 1216–1226. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.5.1216

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Penney, R. K. (1965). Reactive curiosity and manifest anxiety in children. Child Development, 36(3), 697–702.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. New York, NY: Ballantine Books. https://doi.org/10.1037/11168-000

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, M., Bonawitz, E., Shafto, P., Chen, A., & Caglar, L. (2015). Controlling the message: Preschoolers’ use of information to teach and deceive others. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 867. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00867

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, M., Gelman, S. A., & Brickman, D. (2010). Children’s attention to sample composition in learning, teaching and discovery. Developmental Science, 13(3), 421–429. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00896.x

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ruggeri, A., Lombrozo, T., Griffiths, T. L., & Xu, F. (2016). Sources of developmental change in the efficiency of information search. Developmental Psychology, 52(12), 2159.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schulz, L. E., & Bonawitz, E. B. (2007). Serious fun: Preschoolers engage in more exploratory play when evidence is confounded. Developmental Psychology, 43(4), 1045–1050. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.4.1045

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spielberger, C. D., & Starr, L. M. (1994). Curiosity and exploratory behavior. Motivation: Theory and research, 221–243.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stahl, A. E., & Feigenson, L. (2015). Observing the unexpected enhances infants’ learning and exploration. Science, 348(6230), 91–94.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Stahl, A. E., & Feigenson, L. (2017). Expectancy violations promote learning in young children. Cognition, 163, 1–14.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Valdesolo, P., Shtulman, A., & Baron, A. S. (2017). Science is awe-some: The emotional antecedents of science learning. Emotion Review, 9(3), 215–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Schijndel, T. J., Visser, I., van Bers, B. M., & Raijmakers, M. E. (2015). Preschoolers perform more informative experiments after observing theory-violating evidence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 131, 104–119.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72(3), 655–684.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., Kushnir, T., Xu, F., & Brink, K. A. (2016). Infants use statistical sampling to understand the psychological world. Infancy, 21(5), 668–676.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks. Child Development, 75(2), 523–541. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00691.x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Woodward, J. (2005). Making things happen: A theory of causal explanation. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yu, Y., Bonawitz, E., & Shafto, P. (2017). Pedagogical Questions in Parent–Child Conversations. Child development. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12850

  • Yu, Y., Landrum, A., Bonawitz, E., & Shafto, P. (in review) Questioning supports effective transmission of knowledge and increased exploratory learning in pre-kindergarten children.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Thanks to Yue Yu, Yang Yang, Koeun Choi, Joseph Colantonio, Zachary Walden, Carla Macias, Megan Saylor, and Patricia Ganea for helpful feedback on an earlier draft of this chapter. The research presented in this chapter was supported in part by NSF SMA SL-CN (#1640816), NSF SES (#1627971), and the Rutgers University—Newark Chancellor’s Seed Grant Program to EB.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Elizabeth Bonawitz .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Bonawitz, E., Bass, I., Lapidow, E. (2018). Choosing to Learn: Evidence Evaluation for Active Learning and Teaching in Early Childhood. In: Saylor, M., Ganea, P. (eds) Active Learning from Infancy to Childhood. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-77182-3_12

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics