Skip to main content

Demographic and Socioeconomic Characteristics of Persons with Language–Use Limitations, and Special Languages

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Demographic and Socioeconomic Basis of Ethnolinguistics
  • 499 Accesses

Abstract

Some groups in the population have special problems in coding and decoding written and spoken languages and they have had to adopt alternative methods of communication or use irregular speech patterns. Such groups include sensory-impaired persons (i.e., hearing-impaired persons and visually impaired persons), persons having learning disabilities or other speech impediments, such as occur with brain injuries, and persons with neuropsychiatric conditions such as cognitive impairment or schizophrenia. Some groups use special languages for religious ceremonies and rituals that are different from their ordinary speech. Some engage in use of secret languages to conceal their conversation from others, such as the play speech of some children. Many adults address very young children who may be just learning to speak or their pets by a special variation of their ordinary speech. These secret and play languages are here collectively grouped under the heading of “ludlings.” In social networking by computer and related devices young people use a special language for their communication with one another. Finally, artificial languages have been constructed for a variety of reasons. They may have been devised to facilitate international communication between persons who normally speak different languages. Occasionally artificial languages have been created for literary or dramatic purposes, as in science fiction, to represent the speech of beings in outer space. Artificial languages have also been devised to make possible communication between people and machines, and between machines and other machines. Such languages are denoted machine languages.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    A written version of a sign language must represent the configurations and movements of the hands, head, and body in some complex version of ordinary writing. There is an analogy in the notation for dance, which is also three dimensional. Several notation systems for writing sign language have been proposed, but most of them were simple imitations of the writing practices of spoken language. The system called Sign Writing developed by Valerie Sutton uses the spatial relationships of symbols in a two-dimensional “sign box” in order to represent a sign (Thiessen 2011). Sign Writing has a grammar, that is, rules that determine how symbols function and how they combine to form the written signs. According to Thiessen, there are seven major categories of symbols: Hand symbols, movement symbols, a head circle with a set of modifiers, torso and limb symbols, dynamic symbols, punctuation , and sign-spelling notation. The movement symbols describe how the symbols for body parts move and interact with other body parts, and the dynamic symbols provide additional information on the nature of the movement. There are approximately 35,000 symbols, which are variations of 639 base symbols.

  2. 2.

    One hypothesis regarding the progression of language skills in Alzheimer’s Disease is offered by social workers attending patients afflicted with AD. It is that, just as the Activities of Daily Living (ADL—eating, bathing, toileting, transferring, and grooming.) are learned by children in a particular order in early childhood and just as AD patients lose these abilities in reverse order, so language skills were learned in a particular order and, with the progression of AD , language skills are lost in the reverse order in which they were learned.

List of References and Suggested Readings

Sensory Impairments

  • Associated Press/B. Nuckols. (2009, March 26). Fewer than 10% of legally blind Americans read Braille. Printed in USATODAY.com. Accessed on Internet 15 Sept 2016.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker-Shenk, C., & Kyle, J. G. (1990). Research with deaf people. Issues and conflicts. Disability and Society, 5(1), 65–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Branson, J., Miller, D., Marsaja, I. G., & Negara, I. W. (1994). Everyone here speaks sign language too: A deaf village in Bali, Indonesia. In C. Lucas (Ed.), Multicultural aspects of sociolinguistics in deaf communities. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chasin, M. (2015). What is soft speech and how is it dependent on the language being spoken? Hearing Review, 22(3), 14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crews, J. E., & Campbell, V. A. (2004). Vision impairment and hearing loss among community-dwelling older Americans: Implications for health and functioning. American Journal of Public Health, 94(5), 823–829.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deal, J. A., Betz, J., Jaffe, K., Harris, T., et al. (2017). Hearing impairment and incident dementia and cognitive decline in older adults: The health ABC study. Journal of Gerontology A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 72(5), 703–709.

    Google Scholar 

  • Federal Interagency Forum on Aging Related Statistics. (2004). Older Americans 2004: Key indicators of well-being. Washington, DC: Federal Interagency Forum on Aging Related Statistics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Federal Interagency Forum on Aging Related Statistics. (2012). Older Americans 2012: Key indicators of well-being. Washington, DC: Federal Interagency Forum on Aging Related Statistics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herman, R., & Roy, P. (2006). Evidence from the extended use of the BSL receptive skills’ test. Deafness and Education International, 8(1), 33–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holt, J., Hotto, S., & Cole, K. (1994). Demographic aspects of hearing impairment: Questions and answers (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Gallaudet Research Institute.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, R. E. (1994). Sign language and the concept of deafness in a traditional Yucatec Mayan village. In C. J. Erting, R. E. Johnson, D. L. Smith, & B. D. Snider (Eds.), The deaf way: Perspectives from the international conference on deaf culture, 1989. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kegl, J., Senghas, A., & Coppola, M. (1998). Creation through contact: Sign language emergence and sign language change in Nicaragua. In M. DeGraff (Ed.), Comparative grammatical change: The intersection of language acquisition, creole genesis, diachronic syntax (pp. 179–237). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiely, K. M., Mitchell, P., Gopinath, B., Luszcz, M. A., et al. (2016). Estimating the years lived with and without age-related sensory impairment. The Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 71(5), 637–642.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kisch, S. (2008). Deaf discourse: The social construction of deafness in a Bedouin community. Medical Anthropology, 27(3), 283–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kitzel, M. E. (2013). Chasing ancestors: Searching for the roots of American sign language in the Kentish Weald, 1620–1831. PhD thesis, University of Sussex, September 2013. Accessed on internet, Dec 22 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lamb, L., & Wilcox, P. (1988). Acceptance of the American sign language at the University of New Mexico: The history of a process. Sign Language Studies, 17(59), 213–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, D. J., Gómez-Marín, O., Lam, B. L., & Zheng, D. D. (2004). Trends in hearing impairment in United States adults: The National Health Interview Survey, 1986–1995. Journal of Gerontology: Series A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 59(11), 1186–1190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lightfoot, D. (2006). How new languages emerge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. See Chapter 7.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • MacDougall, I. P. (2014). Being deaf/being Maya: Communication and identity negotiation in a Yucatec Maya community. In Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Anthropological Association of America, Washington, DC, December 3–7, 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCaskell, C., Lucas, C., Bayley, R., Hill, J., & Collaborators. (2011). The hidden treasure of black ASL: Its history and structure. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, R. E., Young, T. A., Bachleda, B., & Karchner, M. A. (2006). How many people use ASL in the United States? Why estimates need updating. Sign Language Studies, 6(3), 306–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nyst, V. (2007). A descriptive analysis of Adamorobe sign language (Ghana). Utrecht: LOT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paul, P. V., & Quigley, S. P. (2000). Language and deafness (3rd ed.). San Diego: Singular Publishing Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schein, J. D., & Delk, M. T., Jr. (1974). The deaf population of the United States. Silver Spring: National Association of the Deaf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schubert, C. A., Fischer, M. E., Pinto, A. A., Klein, B. E. K., et al. (2017). Sensory impairments and risk of mortality in older adults. Journal of Gerontology A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 72(5), 710–715.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sellers, F. S. (2012, September 17). Sign language that African-Americans use is different from that of whites. Washiington, DC: Washington Post.

    Google Scholar 

  • Senghas, R. J. (2014). Nicaraguan and international deaf epistemologies: Producing ethnography of emerging agency and identity. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Anthropological Association of America, Washington, DC, December 3–7, 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thiessen, S. (2011). A grammar of sign writing. M.A. Thesis in Linguistics, University of North Dakota.

    Google Scholar 

  • U.S. Census Bureau. (2012). American Factfinder: American Community Survey, 2012. Accessed by Internet: Census.gov.

  • U.S. Census Bureau/He, W., & Larsen, L. J. (2014). Older Americans with a disability, 2008–2012. American Community survey reports ACS-29. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.

    Google Scholar 

  • U.S. National Center for Health Statistics/Schiller, J. S., Lucas, J. W., & Peregoy, J. A. (2012). Summary health statistics for U.S. adults: National Health Interview Survey, 2011. Vital and Health Statistics, Series 10, No 256.

    Google Scholar 

  • U.S. National Center for Health Statistics, Blackwell, D. L., Lucas, J. W., & Clarke, T. C. (2014). Summary health statistics for U.S. adults: National Health Interview survey, 2012. Vital and Health Statistics, Series 10, No 260. Accessed by Internet: CDC/NCHS/NHIS.

    Google Scholar 

  • U.S. National Center for Health Statistics. (2015). Summary health statistics for U.S. adults: National Health Interview Survey, 2014. Accessed by Internet: CDC/NCHS/NHIS.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woll, B., & Sutton-Spence, R. (2011). Sign languages. In J. Simpson (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of applied linguistics (pp. 359–372). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yamada, Y., Denkinger, M. D., Onder, G., van der Roest, H. G., et al. (2016). Joint association of dual sensory impairment and no-activity involvement with 1-year mortality in nursing homes: Results from the SHELTER study. Journal of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences., 71(5), 643–648.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zeshan, U. (2008). Roots, leaves, and branches—the typology of sign languages. TISLR9, a paper presented at the 9th Conference on Theoretical Issues in Sign Language Research, Florianopolis, Brazil, December 2006. In R.M. de Quadros (Ed.), Sign languages: Spinning and unraveling the past, present and future. Petropolis: Editoria Arara Azul, 2008.

    Google Scholar 

Other Physical and Mental Disorders

  • Aiken-Morgan, A. T., Gamaldo, A. A., Sims, P. C., Allaire, J. C., & Whitfield, K. E. (2015). Education desegregation and cognitive change in African-American older adults. Journal of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 70(3), 348–356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bayles, K. A., & Tomoeda, C. K. (1983). Confrontation naming impairment and dementia. Brain and Language, 19, 98–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bayles, K. A., & Tomoeda, C. K. (1991). Caregiver report of prevalence and appearance order of linguistic symptoms in Alzheimer patients. Gerontologist, 31(2), 310–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butterman, E., & Cole, B. (2015). A neuropathway. Penn Arts and Sciences Magazine, Spring/Summer, pp 28–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, D. A., Hebert, L. E., Beckett, L. A., et al. (1997. November). Education and other measures of socioeconomic status and risk of incident Alzheimer disease in a defined population of older persons. Archives of Neurology, 54(11), 1399–1405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glosser, G., & Deser, T. (1992). A comparison of changes in macrolinguistic and microlinguistic aspects of discourse production in normal aging. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 47(4), 266–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glymour, M. M., Kawachi, I., Jencks, C. S., & Berkman, L. S. (2008, June). Does childhood schooling affect old age memory or mental status? Using state schooling laws as natural experiments. J. of Epidemiology and Community Health, 62(6), 532–537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, H. (1994). Conversations with an Alzheimer’s Patient. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ianni, G. (2013). A missed metaphor. Penn Arts and Sciences, SAS Frontiers. Accessed on Internet 23 May 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katzman, R. (1993). Education and the prevalence of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 43(1), 13–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kemper, S., & Lyons, K. (1994). The effects of Alzheimer’s disease on language and communication. In M. L. Hummert, J. Weiman, & J. Nussbaum (Eds.), Interpersonal communication and older adulthood: Interdisciplinary theory and research (pp. 58–82). Newbury Park: Sage.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kemper, S., Greiner, L., Marquis, J., Prenovost, K., & Mitener, T. (2001a). Language decline across the life span: Findings from the nun study. Psychology and Aging, 16, 227–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kemper, S., Thompson, M., & Marquis, J. (2001b). Longitudinal change in language production: Effects of aging and dementia on grammatical complexity and propositional content. Psychology and Aging, 16(4), 600–614.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murdoch, B. E., Chenery, H. J., Wilks, V., & Boyle, R. S. (1987, May). Language disorders in dementia of the Alzheimer type. Brain and Language, 31(1), 122–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newsweek/Bekiempis, V. (2014). Nearly one in five Americans suffers from mental illness each year. February 28, 2014. Accessed at newsweek.com on 28 April 2016.

  • Skirbekk, V., Loichinger, E., & Weber, D. (2012). Variation in cognitive functioning as a refined approach to comparing aging across countries. PNAS, 109(3), 770–774.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snowden, D. A., Kemper, S. J., Mortimer, J. A., Greiner, L. H., et al. (1996). Cognitive ability in early life and cognitive function and Alzheimer’s disease in late life: Findings from the nun study. J. of the American Medical Association, 275, 528–532.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schrauf, R. W., & Muller, N. (Eds.). (2014). Dialogue and dementia: Cognitive and communicative resources for engagement. London: Psychology Press. (part of Taylor and Francis group).

    Google Scholar 

  • U.S. National Institute of Mental Health. (2016). Serious mental illness (SMI) among U.S. adults. Accessed at nimh.nih.gov, 10 April 2016.

  • U.S. National Institute on Aging. (2010). Frontotemporal disorder. Bethesda: National Institute on Aging.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wichmann, M. A., Cruickshanks, K. J., Carlsson, C. M., Chappell, R., et al. (2014, September). Long-term systemic inflammation and cognitive impairment in a population-based cohort. Journal of the American Geriatric Society, 62(9), 1683–1691.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Sacred and Special Languages

  • Baron, N. S. (2008). Always on: Language in an on-line and mobile world. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gershon, I. (2010). The breakup 2.0. Disconnecting our new media. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gleick, J. (2011). The information: A history, a theory, a flood. New York: Pantheon Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarty, T. L. (2010). Native American languages in the United States. In K. Potowski (Ed.), Language diversity in the United States (pp. 47–65). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reader’s Digest. (1978). In J. A. Maxwell (Ed.), America’s fascinating Indian heritage. Pleasantville: The Reader’s Digest Association, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Souza, I. (2010). A phonological description of ‘pet talk’ in Arara. M.A. Thesis in Linguistics at the University of North Dakota.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turaeva, R. (2010/2011). Identification, discrimination and communication: Khorezmian migrants in Tashkent. PhD Thesis, Max Planck Institute for Social Anthropology, Report 2010–2011. Volume I. Halle/Saale.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wikipedia. Pentacostals. Accessed on Internet 15 Jan 2016.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wikipedia. Esperanto. Accessed on Internet 24 July 2016.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer International Publishing AG

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Siegel, J.S. (2018). Demographic and Socioeconomic Characteristics of Persons with Language–Use Limitations, and Special Languages. In: Demographic and Socioeconomic Basis of Ethnolinguistics. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-61778-7_14

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-61778-7_14

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-61776-3

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-61778-7

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics