Skip to main content

Abstract

America was colonized by Asian migrants who moved from northeastern Siberia into North America, either coastally or by an interior route through now-submerged Beringia, and from there spread southward eventually to settle in the entire hemisphere. Linguistic evidence can shed light on when colonization began; whether it was initially coastal, interior, or both; how many distinct populations were involved; and how rapidly the hemisphere was colonized. The time required to generate the historically attested number of languages and language families in the Americas can be estimated; frequencies of structural properties in areally defined linguistic populations can discriminate between populations and point to geographic origins; and attested and straightforwardly reconstructable rates of language spread can be used to estimate rates of migration and demographic spread. On the linguistic evidence, colonization must have begun before the Last Glacial Maximum. There were at least two distinct populations, perhaps corresponding to interior and coastal immigration routes, and in general, coastal immigration seems to have had a stronger and more varied impact on the linguistic population of the Americas than interior immigration did. The immigrants spread at not much over 1 km/year on average (depending on ecology), taking about 7,000 years to reach southern South America. The linguistic dates are robust and based on plentiful and carefully analyzed material, so they cannot be dismissed, although they conflict with the younger ages estimated in genetic, archaeological, and paleoclimatological work.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Here and below I express all dates in calendar years before present.

  2. 2.

    This could have occurred as soon as the corridor supported enough insect and plant life to function as a flyway for migratory waterfowl.

  3. 3.

    See Note 2: migrations of birds would have signaled to hunters that the flyway led to life-supporting terrain.

  4. 4.

    The distance can be walked, of course, in a few years. At issue here is not human walking speed but rates of migration and ethnic spread.

  5. 5.

    Another apparent sprint is the rapid spread of Clovis fluted points across much of North America in some 200 years, a rate of about 24 km/year (Anderson et al. 2005; Waters and Stafford 2007), but in view of its antiquity and the inherent nature of the archaeological record it is likely that sites from both the earliest and latest ends of its spurt are still to be found. In any event the Clovis culture cannot be connected to any language family, so it cannot be compared to the linguistically based rates used here. Hamilton and Buchanan (2010) give an overall spread rate of 7.6 km/year for the entire Clovis spread.

  6. 6.

    This was a series of beeline migrations to known destinations: “Moving in small raiding parties, … followed the Columbia River across the Plateau and through the Cascades to the Pacific” [southward along either the Cascades or the coast] (Golla 2011:257–258). The other rates cited in this paragraph are from Nichols (2008).

  7. 7.

    Bickel and Nichols (2006) use a geographically based definition of the Pacific Rim area: from Pacific (or Pacific-facing) coast and offshore islands inland up to the far side of the major coast range. Pacific Rim traits are those found with significantly higher frequency in the Pacific Rim population than in the adjacent geographical areas (such as intermontane North America, lowland South America, interior and southern New Guinea). See also Nichols et al. (2013). In this approach the definition of the area and the identification of structural properties typical of it are entirely separate.

  8. 8.

    A few tokens can be expected by chance in any continent: Gil (2011) surveys Africa more densely and finds three tokens, but they have no particular geography. He also includes optional classifiers, which increases the density of attestation in the Pacific Rim and expands the area in the directions of its expansion and migrations from it. In Eurasia, many Turkic languages have optional classifiers; ancestral Turkic originated in the vicinity of Manchuria (Janhunen 1996:216), i.e., in or near the Pacific Rim area, and the trait must reflect that origin.

  9. 9.

    Bickel (2013) and Nichols (2010) on different grounds find that even the most stable structural properties are unlikely to last in detectable frequencies as long as 20,000 years. One or another trait might survive much longer in one or another language, but frequencies among that language’s sisters or neighbors are very unlikely to exceed chance, making it impossible to detect ancient families or ancient areas on the strength of just structural typological properties.

References

  • Anderson, D. G., & Gillam, J. C. (2001). Paleoindian interaction and mating networks: Reply to Moore and Moseley. American Antiquity, 66(3), 530–535.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, D. G., Miller, D. S., Yerka, S. J., & Faught, M. K. (2005). Paleoindian database of the Americas: Update 2005. Current Research in the Pleistocene, 22, 91–92.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anthony, D. W. (2007). The horse, the wheel, and Language: How Bronze Age riders from the Eurasian steppes shaped the modern world. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bicho, N. F., Haws, J. A., & Davis, L. G. (Eds.). (2011). Trekking the shore: Changing coastlines and the antiquity of coastal settlement. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bickel, B. (2013). Distributional biases in language families. In B. Bickel, L. Grenoble, D. A. Peterson, & A. Timberlake (Eds.), Language typology and historical contingency (pp. 415–444). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bickel, B., & Nichols, J. (2002). The autotyp research program. Retrieved June 1, 2014, from www.spw.uzh.ch/autotyp/

  • Bickel, B., & Nichols, J. (2006). Oceania, the Pacific Rim, and the theory of linguistic areas. Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, 32S, 3–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blust, R. A. (2009). The austronesian languages. Canberra, Australia: Research School of Pacific and Asian Studies, Australian National University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowern, C., & Koch, H. (2004). Australian languages: Classification and the comparative method. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bulbeck, D. (2007). Where river meets sea: A parsimonious model for Homo sapiens colonization of the Indian Ocean and Sahul. Current Anthropology, 48, 315–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caballero, G., Houser, M. J., Marcus, N., McFarland, T., Pycha, A., Toosarvandani, M., et al. (2008). Nonsyntactic ordering effects in noun incorporation. Linguistic Typology, 12(3), 383–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chang, W., David, H., Chundra, C., & Andrew, G. (2015). Ancestry-constrained phylogenetic analysis supports the steppe chronology of Indo-European origins. Language, 194–244.

    Google Scholar 

  • Darden, B. J. (2001). On the question of the Anatolian origin of Indo-Hittite. In R. Drews (Ed.), Greater Anatolia and the Indo-Hittite language family (pp. 184–228). Washington, DC: Institute for the Study of Man.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denny, J. P. (1991). The Algonquian migration from Plateau to Midwest: Linguistics and archaeology. In W. Cowan (Ed.), Papers of the 22nd Algonquianist Conference (pp. 103–134). Ottawa, Canada: Carleton College.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dillehay, T. D. (1997). Monte Verde: A Late Pleistocene Settlement in Chile. Washington, DC: Smithsonian.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dillehay, T. D., Ramírez, C., Pino, M., Collins, M. B., Rossen, J., & Pino-Navarro, J. D. (2008). Monte Verde: Seaweed, food, medicine, and the peopling of South America. Science, 320, 784–786.

    Google Scholar 

  • Donohue, M., & Denham, T. (2012). Lapita and Proto-Oceanic: Thinking outside the pot? Journal of Pacific History, 47(4), 443–457.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erlandson, J. M., & Braje, T. M. (2011). From Asia to the Americas by boat? Paleogeography, paleoecology, and stemmed points of the northwest Pacific. Quaternary International, 239, 28–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erlandson, J. M., Braje, T. M., & Graham, M. H. (2008a). How old is MV II? Seaweeds, shorelines, and pre-Clovis technology at Monte Verde, Chile. Journal of Island and Coastal Archaeology, 3(2), 277–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erlandson, J. M., Graham, M. H., Bourque, B., Corbett, D., Estes, J. A., & Steneck, R. S. (2007). The Kelp highway hypothesis: Marine ecology, the coastal migration theory, and the peopling of the Americas. Journal of Island and Coastal Archaeology, 2(2), 161–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erlandson, J. M., Moss, M. M., & Des Lauriers, M. (2008b). Life on the edge: Early maritime cultures of the Pacific Coast of North America. Quaternary Science Reviews, 27, 2232–2245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erlandson, J. M., Rick, T. C., Braje, T. J., Casperson, M., Culleton, B., Fulfrost, B., et al. (2011). Paleoindian seafaring, maritime technologies, and coastal foraging on California’s Channel Islands. Science, 331, 1181–1184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fort, J. (2007). Mathematical models of the Neolithic transition: A review for non-mathematicians. British Archaeological Reports, S-1964, 211–216.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fort, J., Toni, P., & Luigi, L.C.-S. (2004). Palaeolithic populations and waves of advance. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 14(1), 53–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fortescue, M. (2005). Comparative Chukotko-Kamchatkan dictionary. Berlin, Germany: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Fortescue, M., Jacobson, S. A., & Kaplan, L. D. (1994). Comparative Eskimo dictionary with Aleut Cognates. Fairbanks, AK: Alaska Native Language Center, University of Alaska Fairbanks.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbons, A. (2014). New sites bring the earliest Americans out of the shadows. Science, 344, 567–568.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gil, D. (2011). Numeral classifiers. In M. S. Dryer & M. Haspelmath (Eds.), The world atlas of language structures online. Leipzig, Germany: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. Retrieved May 14, 2014, from http://wals.info/chapter/55

  • Gilbert, M. T. P., Jenkins, D. L., Götherstrom, A., Naveran, N., Sanchez, J. J., Hofreiter, M., et al. (2008). DNA from Pre-Clovis human coprolites in Oregon, North America. Science, 320, 786–789.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Golla, V. (2011). California Indian languages. Berkeley, LA: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halligan, J. (2014). Geoarchaeological interpretations of reported pre-Clovis components in the Aucilla River, Florida. Presented at Society for American Archaeology annual meeting, Austin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, M. J., & Buchanan, B. (2010). Archaeological support for the three-stage expansion of modern humans across northeastern Eurasia and into the Americas. PLoS One, 5(8), e12472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, J. H. (2004, February). Language spread among hunter-gatherers: The North American evidence. Presented at Conference in Honor of Murray B. Emeneau, Berkeley, CA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffecker, J. F., Elias, S. A., & O’Rourke, D. H. (2014). Out of Beringia? Science, 343, 979–980.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Janhunen, J. (1996). Manchuria: An ethnic history. Helsinki, Finland: Suomalais-ugrilainen Seura.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirch, P. V. (1997). The Lapita peoples: Ancestors of the oceanic world. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirch, P. V. (2000). On the road of the winds: An archaeological history of the Pacific Islands before European contact. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirch, P. V. (2010). The peopling of the Pacific: A holistic anthropological perspective. Annual Review of Anthropology, 39, 131–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Madsen, D. B. (Ed.). (2004). Entering America: Northeast Asia and Beringia before the last glacial maximum. Salt Lake City, UT: University of Utah Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McConvell, P. (1996). Backtracking to Babel: The chronology of Pama-Nyungan expansion in Australia. Archaeology of Oceania, 31, 125–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, J. H. (1987). The Cheyenne nation: A social and demographic history. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore, J. H. (2001). Ethnogenetic patterns in native North America. In J. Terrell (Ed.), Archaeology, language, and history: Essays on culture and ethnicity. Westport, CT: Bergin & Garvey.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nichols, J. (2000). Estimating dates of early American colonization events. In C. Renfrew, A. McMahon, & L. Trask (Eds.), Time depth in historical linguistics (Vol. 2, pp. 643–663). Cambridge, UK: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nichols, J. (2008). Language spread rates as indicators of glacial-age peopling of the Americas. Current Anthropology, 49(6), 1109–1117. Plus supporting online material.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nichols, J. (2010). Language families, macroareas, and contact. In R. Hickey (Ed.), The handbook of language contact (pp. 361–379). London: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nichols, J., & Bickel, B. (2011). Possessive classification. In M. Dryer & M. Haspelmath (Eds.), The world atlas of language structures online. Leipzig, Germany: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. Retrieved May 11, 2014, from http://wals.info/chapter/59

  • Nichols, J., & Peterson, D. A. (1996). The Amerind personal pronouns. Language, 72, 336–371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nichols, J., & Peterson, D.A. (2011). Personal pronouns: M-T and N-M patterns. In M. Dryer and M. Haspelmath, (Eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Leipzig, Germany: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. Retrieved May 11, 2014 from http://wals.info/chapter/137.

  • Nichols, J., Witzlack-Makarevich, A., Bickel, B. (2013). The Autotyp genealogy and geography. Retrieved May 29, 2014, from www.spw.uzh.ch/autotyp/

  • Norman, J. L. (1988). Chinese. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perego, U. A., Achilli, A., Angerhofer, N., Accetturo, M., Pala, M., Olivieri, A., et al. (2009). Distinctive Paleo-Indian migration routes from Beringia marked by two rare mtDNA haplogroups. Current Biology, 19(1), 1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramsey, S. R. (1987). The languages of China. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross, M. D., Pawley, A. K., & Osmond, M. (Eds.). (2008). The Lexicon of Proto Oceanic, 3: Plants. (Pacific Linguistics.). Canberra, Australia: Research School of Pacific and Asian Studies, Australian National University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schurr, T. G. (2004). The peopling of the New World: Perspectives from molecular anthropology. Annual Review of Anthropology, 33, 551–583.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tamm, E., Kivisild, T., Reidla, M., Metspalu, M., Smith, D. G., Mulligan, C. J., et al. (2007). Beringian standstill and spread of Native American founders. PLoS One, 2(9), e829.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waters, M. R., & Stafford, T. W., Jr. (2007). Redefining the age of Clovis: Implications for the peopling of the Americas. Science, 315, 1122–1126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waters, M. R., Stafford, T. W., Jr., McDonald, H. G., Gustafson, C., Rasmussen, M., Cappellini, E., et al. (2011). Pre-Clovis mastodon hunting 13,800 years ago at the Manis site, Washington. Science, 334, 351–353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Willerslev, E., Davison, J., Moora, M., Zobel, M., Coissac, E., Edwards, M. E., et al. (2014). Fifty thousand years of Arctic vegetation and megafaunal diet. Nature, 506, 47–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I thank the Embassy of Kazakhstan, the Permanent Delegation of Kazakhstan to UNESCO, and the Harriman Institute (Columbia University) for making possible the second Great Migrations conference, which among other boons suggested some of the new lines of thought in this work. The research reported here was supported in part by the NSF and the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig; support for the Autotyp database has additionally been provided by the Committee on Research of the University of California, Berkeley, the University of Leipzig, and the University of Zürich.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Johanna Nichols .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Nichols, J. (2015). How America Was Colonized: Linguistic Evidence. In: Frachetti, M., Spengler III, R. (eds) Mobility and Ancient Society in Asia and the Americas. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-15138-0_9

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics