Skip to main content

Verb Representation and Thinking-for-Speaking Effects in Spanish–English Bilinguals

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Cognitive Science Perspectives on Verb Representation and Processing

Abstract

Speakers of English habitually encode motion events using manner-of-motion verbs (e.g., spin, roll, slide) whereas Spanish speakers rely on path-of-motion verbs (e.g., enter, exit, approach). Here, we ask whether the language-specific verb representations used in encoding motion events induce different modes of “thinking-for-speaking” in Spanish–English bilinguals. That is, assuming that the verb encodes the most salient information in the clause, do bilinguals find the path of motion to be more salient than manner of motion if they had previously described the motion event using Spanish versus English? In our study, Spanish–English bilinguals described a set of target motion events in either English or Spanish and then participated in a nonlinguistic similarity judgment task in which they viewed the target motion events individually (e.g., a ball rolling into a cave) followed by two variants a “same-path” variant such as a ball sliding into a cave or a “same-manner” variant such as a ball rolling away from a cave). Participants had to select one of the two variants that they judged to be more similar to the target event: The event that shared the same path of motion as the target versus the one that shared the same manner of motion. Our findings show that bilingual speakers were more likely to classify two motion events as being similar if they shared the same path of motion and if they had previously described the target motion events in Spanish versus in English. Our study provides further evidence for the “thinking-for-speaking” hypothesis by demonstrating that bilingual speakers can flexibly shift between language-specific construals of the same event “on-the-fly.”

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Japanese–English participants told the story once in Japanese and once in English.

  2. 2.

    English speakers are not predicted to differ in their similarity judgments based on whether they heard telic versus atelic descriptions because telicity does not influence the ability of manner verbs to combine with path phrases in English to the extent that it does in Spanish.

  3. 3.

    We thank one of the anonymous reviewers for pointing out this possibility.

  4. 4.

    We thank one of the anonymous reviewers for pointing out this possibility.

References

  • Aske, J. (1989). Path predicates in English and Spanish: A closer look. In K. Hall, M. Meacham, & R. Shapiro (Eds.), Proceedings of the fifteenth annual meeting of the Berkeley linguistics society (pp. 1–14). Berkeley: Berkeley Linguistics Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Athanasopolous, P. (2007). Interaction between grammatical categories and cognition in bilinguals: The role of proficiency, cultural immersion, and language of instruction. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22, 689–699.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baayen, H. (2008). Analyzing linguistic data: A practical introduction to statistics using R. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 8) (pp. 47–89). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, S., & Snyder, W. (2001). Complex predicates and goal PP’s: Evidence for a semantic parameter. In A. H.-J. Do, L. Dominguez, & A. Johansen (Eds.), Proceedings of the 25th Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 114–122). Somerville: Cascadilla Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berlin, B., & Kay, P. (1969). Basic color terms: Their universality and evolution. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Billman, D., & Krych, M. (1998). Path and manner verbs in action: Effects of “skipping” or “existing” on event memory. Proceedings of the Cognitive Science Society, 20, 156–161.

    Google Scholar 

  • Billman, D., Swilley, A., & Krych, M. (2000). Path and manner priming: Verb production and event recognition. Proceedings of the Cognitive Science Society, 22, 615–620.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bohnemeyer, J., Eisenbeiss, S., & Narasimhan, B. (2001). Event Triads. In S. C. Levinson & N. Enfield (Eds.), Manual for the field season 2001 (pp. 100–114). Nijmegen: Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bohnemeyer, J., Eisenbeiss, S., & Narasimhan, B. (2006). Ways to go: Methodological considerations in Whorfian studies on motion events. Essex Research Reports in Linguistics, 50, 1–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boroditsky, L. (2001). Does language shape thought? English and Mandarin speakers’ conceptions of time. Cognitive Psychology, 43, 1–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A., & Gullberg, M. (2008). Bidirectional crosslinguistic influence in L1-L2 encoding of Manner in speech and gesture: A study of Japanese speakers of English. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 30, 225–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cook, V. J., Bassetti, B., Kasai, C., Sasaki, M., & Takahashi, J. A. (2006). Do bilinguals have different concepts? The case of shape and material in Japanese L2 users of English. International Journal of Bilingualism, 10, 137–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erlam, R. (2006). Elicited imitation as a measure of L2 implicit knowledge: An empirical validation study. Applied Linguistics, 27, 464–491.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finkbeiner, M., Nicol, J., Greth, D., & Nakamura, K. (2002). The role of language in memory for actions. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 31, 447–457.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gennari, S. P., Sloman, S. A., Malt, B. C., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). Motion events in language and cognition. Cognition, 83, 49–79.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D., Imai, M., & Boroditsky, L. (2002). As time goes by: Evidence for two systems in processing space time metaphors. Language & Cognitive Processes, 17, 537–565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gullberg, M. (2011). Thinking, speaking, and gesturing about motion in more than one language. In Pavlenko A. (Ed.) Thinking and speaking in two languages (pp. 143-169). Multilingual Matters.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gullberg, M., & Indefrey, P. (2003). Language background questionnaire. Developed in the dynamics of multilingual processing. Nijmegen, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heider, E. R. (1972). Universals in color naming and memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 93, 10–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Imai, M., & Gentner, D. (1997). A crosslinguistic study of early word meaning: Universal ontology and linguistic influence. Cognition, 62, 169–200.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kita, S., & Özyürek, A. (2003). What does cross-linguistic variation in semantic coordination of speech and gesture reveal? Evidence for an interface representation of spatial thinking and speaking. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 16–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lai, V. T. (2002). The Processing of Temporal Perspectives in Mandarin Chinese. Unpublished MA thesis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lai, V. T. (2005). Language experience influences in the conceptualization of time metaphor. Proceedings of the II Conference on Metaphor in Language and Thought (pp. 1–16). Rio de Janeiro: Universidade Federal Fluminense

    Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W. J. M. (1996). Perspective taking and ellipsis in spatial descriptions. In P. Bloom, M. A. Peterson, M. F. Garrett, & L. Nadels (Eds.), Language and space (pp. 77–107). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin, B., & Rapoport, T. (1988). Lexical subordination. Papers from the Twenty Fourth Regional Meeting, Chicago Linguistic Society (pp. 275–289). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin, B., & Rappaport Hovav, M. (1999). Two structures for compositionally derived events. Proceedings of SALT 9, Cornell Linguistics Circle Publications, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY, pp. 199–223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin, B., & Rappaport Hovav, M. (1995). Unaccusativity: At the syntax–lexical semantics interface. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, S. C. (1996). Relativity and spatial conception and description. In J. J. Gumperz & S. C. Levinson (Eds.), Rethinking linguistic relativity (pp. 177–202). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, S. C., Kita, S., Haun, D. B. M., & Rasch, B. H. (2002). Returning the tables: Language affects spatial reasoning. Cognition, 84, 155–188.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Li, P., & Gleitman, L. (2002). Turning the tables: Language and spatial reasoning. Cognition, 83, 265–294.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loftus, E., & Palmer, J. (1974). Reconstruction of automobile destruction: An example of the interaction between language and memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 585–589.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lucy, J. A. (1996). The scope of linguistic relativity: An analysis and review of empirical research. In J. J. Gumperz & S. C. Levinson (Eds.), Rethinking linguistic relativity. Studies in the social and cultural foundations of language, No. 17. (pp. 37–69). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGlone, M. S., & Harding, J. L. (1998). Back (or forward?) to the future: The role of perspective in temporal language comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 1211–1223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Narasimhan, B. (2003). Motion events and the lexicon: The case of Hindi. Lingua, 113(2), 123–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papafragou, A., Massey, C., & Gleitman, L. (2002). Shake, rattle, ‘n’ roll: The representation of motion in language and cognition. Cognition, 84, 189–219.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Papafragou, A., Hulbert, J. C., & Trueswell, J. (2008). Does language guide event perception? Evidence from eye movements. Cognition, 108, 155–184.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pederson, E. (1995). Language as context, language as means: Spatial cognition and habitual language use. Cognitive Linguistics, 6, 33–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pederson, E., Danziger, E., Wilkins, D., Levinson, S., Kita, S., & Senft, G. (1998). Semantic typology and spatial conceptualization. Language, 74, 557–589.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D. I. (1987). Thinking for speaking. Proceedings of the thirteenth annual meeting of the Berkeley linguistics society (pp. 435–444), Berkeley Linguistics Society, Berkeley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D. I. (1996a). From “thought and language” to “thinking to speaking”. In J. J. Gumperz & S. C. Levinson (Eds.), Rethinking linguistic relativity (pp. 70–96). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D. I. (1996b). Two ways to travel: Verbs of motion in English and Spanish. In M. S. Shibatani & S. A. Thompson (Eds.), Grammatical constructions: Their form and meaning (pp. 195–220). Oxford: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D. I., & Hoiting, N. (1994). Reference to movement in spoken and signed languages: Typological considerations. Proceedings of the twentieth annual meeting of the Berkeley linguistic society (pp. 487–505), Berkeley Linguistics Society, Berkeley.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Stutterheim, C., & Nüse, R. (2003). Processes of conceptualization in language production: Language-specific perspectives and event construal. Linguistics, 41, 851–881.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Talmy, L. (1985). Lexicalization patterns: Semantic structure in lexical forms. In T. Shopen (Ed.), Language typology and syntactic description 3: Grammatical categories and the lexicon (pp. 57–149). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Talmy, L. (2000). Toward a cognitive semantics: Typology and process in concept structuring (pp. 21–146). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Talmy, L. (2007). Lexical typologies. In T. Shopen (Ed.), Language typology and syntactic description volume 3, grammatical categories and the lexicon (pp. 166–168). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whorf, B. L. (1956). Language, thought, and reality. Boston: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkins, D. P. & Hill, D. (1995). When GO means COME: Questioning the basicness of basic motion verbs. Cognitive Linguistics, 6, 209–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zubizarreta, M. L & Oh, E. (2007). On the syntactic composition of manner and motion. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank the editors of the volume and two anonymous reviewers for their valuable comments. We also thank the members of the Language, Cognition and Development Lab at the University of Colorado, Boulder, for their feedback and the research assistants Anwen Fredriksen and Elizabeth Saade for their help in running the experiments.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Vicky T. Lai .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Lai, V., Narasimhan, B. (2015). Verb Representation and Thinking-for-Speaking Effects in Spanish–English Bilinguals. In: de Almeida, R., Manouilidou, C. (eds) Cognitive Science Perspectives on Verb Representation and Processing. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-10112-5_11

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics