Skip to main content

Cognitive–Emotional Development from Adolescence to Adulthood

  • Chapter
Integrating Emotions and Cognition Throughout the Lifespan

Abstract

This chapter presented aspects of cognitive–emotional development from adolescence to adulthood. Adolescence brings major advances in cognitive development, advances that Piaget refers to as “formal operations”—the ability to deal with propositions that are highly abstract rather than concrete. Piaget assumed that formal operations was the penultimate level of cognitive development, but since about 1980, a number of researchers offered critiques of the implication that cognitive growth abated in adolescence. Instead, a number of proposals appeared that, though independent, converged on an extension of Piaget’s theory. These extensions proposed that thinking needs to be integrated with emotional and pragmatic aspects, rather than only dealing with the purely abstract. This integration of emotion and cognition provides youth with a more flexible understanding of emotions—an understanding that reaches its heights only in mature adulthood under the form of post-formal thinking. The chapter gives definitions and examples of such post-formal thinking in the realm of emotional complexity from preadolescence to late adulthood.

A second major theme of the chapter follows up on Erik Erikson’s theory, stating that in order to reach cognitive–emotional maturity, adolescents require an alliance with more mature adults who can provide admired generative models who can guide the young towards affirmation of important goals. The current chapter continues, however, in discussing the fact many adults may not be effective models of generative ideals, because they themselves may suffer from long-term consequences of problematic upbringing that carry into adulthood an early acquired incapacity of dealing with affect and integrating it with emotion. As a result, they pass on some of their own lack of integration to the younger generation as exemplified, for example, in the gender disparity related to depression. The chapter discusses research demonstrating such failures to integrate at different stages of the life span—failures which may result in incapacity to form a coherent narrative of one’s life (or even a coherent vision of oneself), and to block out memories of important emotionally salient events altogether. As a case in point, examples of functional Amnesia in adults are presented.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    A syllogism is a formal argument in logic that is formed by two statements and a conclusion which must be true if the two statements are true (Merriam Webster).

  2. 2.

    Amnesia.

  3. 3.

    Person retreats from affect to using words and abstractions that are often inappropriate to the specific situation. Uses jargon and is pseudointellectual.

  4. 4.

    The person ignores past or present facts that would be unpleasant to acknowledge and instead focuses on the benign.

References

  • Adams, C., Labouvie-Vief, G., Hakim-Larson, J., DeVoe, M., & Hayden, M. (1988). Modes of thinking and problem solving: Developmental transitions from preadolescence to middle adulthood. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan. Unpublished Manuscript.

    Google Scholar 

  • Armon, C. (1984). Ideals of the good life and moral judgment. In M. L. Commons, F. A. Richards, & C. Armon (Eds.), Beyond formal operations: Late adolescent and adult cognitive development (pp. 357–380). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arnett, J. J. (2004). Emerging adulthood: The winding road from the late teens through the twenties. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Basseches, M. A. (1984). Dialectical thinking as a metasystematic form of cognitive organization. In M. L. Commons, F. A. Richards, & C. Armon (Eds.), Beyond formal operations: Late adolescent and adult cognitive development (pp. 216–238). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blanchard-Fields, F. (1986). Reasoning on social dilemmas varying in emotional saliency: An adult developmental perspective. Psychology and Aging, 1, 322–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blanchard-Fields, F. (1989). Postformal reasoning and a socioemotional context. In M. L. Commons, J. D. Sinnott, F. A. Richards, & C. Armon (Eds.), Adult development: Vol. 1. Comparisons and applications of developmental models (pp. 73–93). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blasi, A., & Hoeffel, E. C. (1974). Adolescence and formal operations. Human Development, 17, 344–363.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bogutyn, T., Kokoszka, A., Palczynski, J., & Holas, P. (1999). Defense mechanisms in alexithymia. Psychological Reports, 84, 183–187.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bretherton, I., Fritz, J., Zahn-Waxler, C., & Ridgeway, D. (1986). Learning to talk about emotions: A functionalist perspective. Child Development, 57, 529–548.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cabeza, R., & St Jacques, P. (2007). Functional neuroimaging of autobiographical memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(5), 219–227.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (2002). A developmental psychopathology perspective on drug abuse. In M. D. Glantz & C. R. Hartel (Eds.), Drug abuse: Origins and interventions (pp. 97–117). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Commons, F. A. R., & Armon, C. (Eds.). (1984). Beyond formal operations: Late adolescent and adult cognitive development. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cytrynbaum, S., Blum, L., Patrick, R., Stein, J., Wasner, D., & Wilk, C. (1980). Midlife development: A personality and social systems perspective. In L. W. Poon (Ed.), Aging in the 1980’s: Psychological issues (pp. 463–474). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • DeLong, H. (1970). A profile of mathematical logic. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diehl, M., Chui, H., Hay, E. L., Lumley, M. A., Grühn, D., & Labouvie-Vief, G. (2013). Change in coping and defense mechanisms across adulthood: Longitudinal findings in a European American sample. Developmental Psychology, 50(2), 634–648. doi:10.1037/a0033619.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Donaldson, M., & Westerman, M. A. (1986). Development of children’s understanding of ambivalence and causal theories of emotion. Developmental Psychology, 22, 655–662.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erikson, E. (1968). Identity: Youth and crisis. New York: W.W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fink, G. R., Markowitsch, H. J., Reinkemeirer, M., Bruckbauer, T., Kessler, J., & Heiss, W. D. (1996). Cerebral representation of one’s own past: Neural networks involved in autobiographical memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 16, 4275–4282.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Florian, V., & Mikulincer, M. (1997). Fear of death and the judgment of social transgression: A multidimensional test of terror management theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 369–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fredrickson, B. L. (2013). Positive emotions broaden and build. In E. Ashby Plant & P. G. Devine (Eds.), Advances on experimental social psychology (Vol. 47, pp. 1–53). Burlington: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S. (1957). The id and the ego. In J. Rickman (Ed.), A general selection from the works of Sigmund Freud (pp. 210–235). Garden City, NY: Doubleday (Original work published 1823).

    Google Scholar 

  • Giedd, J. N., Blumenthal, J., Jeffries, N. O., Castellanos, F. X., Liu, H., Zijdenbos, A., et al. (1999). Brain development during childhood and adolescence: A longitudinal MRI study. Nature Neuroscience, 2(10), 861–863.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haan, N. (1977). Coping and defending: Processes of self-environment organization. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, P. L. (1989). Children and emotion. London: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, P. L., Olthof, T., & Terwogt, M. (1981). Children’s knowledge of emotion. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 22, 247–261.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harter, S., & Buddin, B. J. (1987). Children’s understanding of the simultaneity of emotions: A five-stage developmental acquisition sequence. Developmental Psychology, 23, 388–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harter, S., & Monsour, A. (1992). Developmental analysis of conflict caused by opposing attributes in the adolescent self-portrait. Developmental Psychology, 28, 251–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, S. T. (1976). Loevinger’s model and measure of ego development: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 83, 928–955.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heilbrun, C. (1988). Writing a woman’s life. New York: Ballantine Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Helson, R., & Wink, P. (1992). Personality change in women from the early 40s to the early 50s. Psychology and Aging, 7(1), 46–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hollingworth, L. (1926). Gifted children: Their nature and nurture. New York: Macmillan.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hy, L. X., & Loevinger, J. (1996). Measuring ego development (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ihilevich, D., & Gleser, G. C. (1986). Defense mechanisms: Their classification, correlates, and measurement with the Defense Mechanisms Inventory. Owosso, MI: DMI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M., Daubman, K. A., & Nowicki, G. P. (1987). Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1122–1131.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M., Niedenthal, P., & Cantor, N. (1992). An influence of positive affect on social categorization. Motivation and Emotion, 16, 65–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1892). The stream of consciousness. (First published in Psychology, Ch XI. Cleveland & New York, World)

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, L. D., O’Malley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., & Schulenberg, J. E. (2004). Monitoring the future national survey results on drug use, 1975–2003: Volume II, College students and adults ages 19–45 (NIH Publication No. 04-5508). Bethesda: National Institute on Drug Abuse.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jung, C. G. (1933). Modern man in search of a soul (R. F. C. Hull, Trans.). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keating, D. O. (2004). Cognitive and brain development. In R. M. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (pp. 45–84). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kegan, J. (1982). The evolving self. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kerr, B. A. (1987). Smart girls, gifted women. Columbus, OH: Ohio.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitchener, K. S., & King, P. M. (1981). Reflective judgement: Concepts of justification and their relationship to age and education. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 2, 89–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klaczynki, P., & Gordon, D. (1996). Everyday statistical reasoning during adolescence and young adulthood: Motivational, general ability, and developmental influences. Child Development, 67, 2873–2891.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klaczynski, P. A. (1997). Bias in adolescents’ everyday reasoning and its relationship with intellectual ability, personal theories, and self-serving motivation. Developmental Psychology, 33, 273–283.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kohlberg, L. (1969). Stage and sequence: The cognitive-developmental approach to socialization. In D. A. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and research (pp. 347–380). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kopelman, M. D., Christensen, H., Puffett, A., & Stanhope, N. (1994). The great escape: A neuropsychological study of psychogenic amnesia. Neuropsychologia, 32, 675–691.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuther, T. L., & Higgins-D’Allessandro, A. (2001). Attitudinal and normative predictors of alcohol consumption among older adolescents and young adults. Journal of Research in Adolescence, 24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Labouvie-Vief, G. (1980). Beyond formal operations: Uses and limits of pure logic in life-span development. Human Development, 23, 141–161.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Labouvie-Vief, G. (1982). Dynamic development and mature autonomy: A theoretical prologue. Human Development, 25, 161–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Labouvie-Vief, G. (1984). Logic and self-regulation from youth to maturity: A model. In M. L. Commons & S. Bennack (Eds.), Post-formal operations (pp. 158–179). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Labouvie-Vief, G. (1994). Psyche and Eros: Mind and gender in the life course. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Labouvie-Vief, G. (1996). Knowing and relating: The lost dimension of knowledge in Piaget’s theory. Culture and Psychology, 2, 323–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Labouvie-Vief, G. (2001). Ego development in adulthood. In N. J. Smelser & P. B. Baltes (Eds.), International encyclopedia of the social and behavioral sciences (pp. 4361–4365). Oxford: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Labouvie-Vief, G., DeVoe, M., & Bulka, D. (1989). Speaking about feelings: Conceptions of emotion across the life span. Psychology and Aging, 4, 425–437.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Labouvie-Vief, G., Hakim-Larson, J., & Hobart, C. (1987). Age, ego level, and the life-span development of coping and defense processes. Psychology and Aging, 2, 286–293.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Labouvie-Vief, G., & Schell, D. (1982). Learning and memory in later life: A developmental perspective. In B. Wolman & G. Stricker (Eds.), Handbook of developmental psychology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, D. J., Darrow, C. N., Klein, E. B., Levinson, M. H., & McKee, B. (1978). The seasons of a man’s life. New York: Ballantine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, M., Sullivan, M. W., Stanger, C., & Weiss, M. (1989). Self-development and self-conscious emotions. Child Development, 60, 146–156.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Linn, M. C., & Siegel, H. (1984). Postformal reasoning: A philosophical model. In M. L. Commons & S. Bennack (Eds.), Post-formal operations (pp. 249–257). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loevinger, J. (1969). Theories of ego development. In L. Breger (Ed.), Clinical-cognitive psychology: Models and integrations (pp. 83–135). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loevinger, J., & Wessler, R. (1978). Measuring ego development. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Macmurray, J. (1978). The self as agent. Atlantic Highlands, NJ: Humanities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markowitsch, H. J. (2002). Functional retrograde amnesia—Mnestic block syndrome. Cortex, 38(4), 651–654.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Markowitsch, H. J., & Staniloiu, A. (2012). The impairment of recollection in functional amnesic states. Cortex, 2012, 1–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markowitsch, H. J., Kessler, J., Van der Ven, C., Weber-Luxenburger, G., & Heiss, W.-D. (1998). Psychic trauma causing grossly reduced brain metabolism and cognitive deterioration. Neuropsychologia, 36, 77–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Markowitsch, H. J., Kessler, J., Weber-Luxenburger, G., Van der Ven, C., & Heiss, W.-D. (2000). Neuroimaging and behavioral correlates of recovery from ‘Mnestic Block Syndrome’ and other cognitive deteriorations. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology, 13, 60–66.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Masten, A. S., Hubbard, J. J., Gest, S. D., Tellegen, A., Garmezy, N., & Ramirez, M. (1999). Competence in the context of adversity: Pathways to resilience and maladaptation from childhood to late adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 11, 143–169.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mikulincer, M. (1998). Adult attachment style and affect regulation: Strategic variation in self-appraisals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 420–435.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mikulincer, M., Florian, V., & Tolmarz, R. (1990). Attachment styles and fear of personal death: A case study of affect regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 273–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mikulincer, M., Orbach, I., & Iavneli, D. (1998). Adult attachment style and affect regulation: Strategic variations in subjective self-other similarity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 436–448.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neugarten, B. L., Crotty, W. J., & Tobin, S. S. (1964). Personality types in an aged population. In B. L. Neugarten (Ed.), Personality in middle and late life. New York: Atherton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1990). Sex differences in depression. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perry, W. G. (1968). Forms of intellectual and ethical development in the college years. New York: Holt, Rineheart, & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1967). Six psychological studies (M. Gabain, Trans.). New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1971). Biology and knowledge [Biologie et connaissance; essai sur les relations entre les régulations organiques et les processus cognitifs (1967)]. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1981). Intelligence and affectivity: Their relationship during child development (T. A. Brown & C. E. Kaegi, Trans.). Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1998). Insight and illusions of philosophy. New York: Routledge (Reprinted Edition).

    Google Scholar 

  • Pysczynki, T., Greenberg, J., & Solomon, S. (1999). A dual-process model of defense against conscious and unconscious death-related thoughts: An extension of terror management theory. Psychological Review, 106, 835–845.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reinherz, H. Z., Giacona, R. M., Carmola, A. M., Wasserman, M. S., Silverman, A. B., & Burton, L. M. (1999). Major depression in the transition to adulthood: Risks and impairments. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108, 500–510.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rich, A. C. (1969). Leaflets. New York: W.W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., & Evans, D. E. (2000). Temperament and personality: Origins and outcomes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 122–135.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. L., & Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness and human potentials: A review on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annual Review of Psychology, 52(1), 141–166.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saarni, C. (1984). Observing children’s use of display rules: Age and sex differences. Child Development, 55, 1504–1513.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schore, A. N. (2003a). Affect dysregulation and disorders of the self. New York: W.W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schore, A. N. (2003b). Affect regulation and the repair of the self. New York: W.W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schulenberg, J. E., & Zarrett, N. R. (2006). Mental health during emerging adulthood: Continuity and discontinuity in courses, causes, and functions. In J. J. Arnett & J. L. Tanner (Eds.), Emerging adults in America: Coming of age in the 21st century (pp. 135–172). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Sinnott, J. D. (1984). Postformal reasoning: The relativistic stage. In M. L. Commons, F. A. Richards, & C. Armon (Eds.), Beyond formal operations: Late adolescent and adult cognitive development (pp. 298–325). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steinberg, L. (2005). Cognitive and affective development in adolescence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(2), 69–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Terman, L. M., & Oden, M. (1925). Genetic studies of genius: Vol. 1. Mental and physical traits of a thousand gifted children. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomas-Anterion, C., Gudj, E., Decousus, M., & Laurent, B. (2012). Can we see personal identity loss? A functional imaging study of typical ‘hysterical’ amnesia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 81(4), 468–469.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. (1910). Educational psychology (2nd ed.). New York: Teachers College, Columbia University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (1989, August). Origins and directions for the concept of infant intersubjectivity. SRCD Newsletter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trull, T. J. (2001). Structural relations between borderline personality disorder features and putative etiological correlates. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 471–481.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic memory: From mind to brain. Annual Reviews of Psychology, 53, 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E. (2005). Episodic memory and autonoesis: Uniquely human? In H. S. Tarrace & J. Metcalfe (Eds.), The missing link in cognition: Self-knowing consciousness in man and animals (pp. 3–56). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vaillant, G. E., & McArthur, C. C. (1972). Natural history of male psychological health: The adult life cycle from eighteen to fifty. Seminars in Psychiatry, 4, 415–427.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vanheule, S., Desmet, M., Meganck, R., & Bogaerts, S. (2007). Alexithymia and interpersonal problems. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 63, 109–117.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wainryb, C., Shaw, L., Laupa, M., & Smith, K. R. (2001). Children’s, adolescents’, and young adults, thinking about different types of disagreements. Developmental Psychology, 37, 373–386.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Werner, H. (1957). Comparative psychology of mental development. Oxford: International Universities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ziadni, M., Jasinski, M. J., Labouvie-Vief, G., & Lumley, M. A. Affective self-regulation: Relationships among alexithymia, defenses, and ego maturity; and prediction of depression and well-being after six years. Manuscript under review.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Labouvie-Vief, G. (2015). Cognitive–Emotional Development from Adolescence to Adulthood. In: Integrating Emotions and Cognition Throughout the Lifespan. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-09822-7_6

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics