Skip to main content

Cognitive Archaeology and the Cognitive Sciences

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Human Paleoneurology

Part of the book series: Springer Series in Bio-/Neuroinformatics ((SSBN,volume 3))

Abstract

Cognitive archaeology uses cognitive and psychological models to interpret the archaeological record. This chapter outlines several components that may be essential in building effective cognitive archaeological arguments. It also presents a two-stage perspective for the development of modern cognition, primarily based upon the work of Coolidge and Wynn. The first describes the transition from arboreal to terrestrial life in later Homo and the possible cognitive repercussions of terrestrial sleep. The second stage proposes that a genetic event may have enhanced working memory in Homo sapiens (specifically in terms of Baddeley’s multicomponent working memory model). The present chapter also reviews the archaeological and neurological bases for modern thinking, and the latter arguments are primarily grounded in the significance of the morphometric rescaling of the parietal lobes, which appears to have distinguished Homo sapiens from Neandertals.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Aboitiz F, Aboitiz S, Garcia R (2010) The phonological loop: a key innovation in human evolution. Curr Anthropol 51:S55–S65

    Google Scholar 

  • Adams AM, Gathercole S (2000) Limitations in working memory: implications for language development. Int J Lang Commun Disord 35:95–116

    Google Scholar 

  • Addis DR, Wong AT, Schacter DL (2007) Remembering the past and imagining the future: common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration. Neuropsychologia 45:1363–1377

    Google Scholar 

  • Allison T, Cicchetti DV (1976) Sleep in mammals: ecological and constitutional correlates. Science 194:732–734

    Google Scholar 

  • Ambrose S (2010) Coevolution of composite-tool technology, constructive memory, and language: implications for the evolution of modern human behavior. In: Wynn T, Coolidge F (eds) Working memory: beyond language and symbolism. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Antón SC (2003) Natural history of Homo erectus. Yearb Phys Anthropol 46:126–170

    Google Scholar 

  • Antón SC, Swisher CC (2004) Early dispersals of Homo from Africa. Annu Rev Anthropol 33:271–296

    Google Scholar 

  • Antón S, Leonard W, Robertson M (2002) An ecomorphological model of the initial hominid dispersal from Africa. J Hum Evol 43:773–785

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley A, Hitch GJ (1974) Working memory. In: Bower GA (ed) Recent advances in learning and motivation. Academic Press, New York, pp 47–90

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley A, Logie R (1999) Working memory: the multiple-component model. In: Miyake A, Shah P (eds) Models of working memory: mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 28–61

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley A (2000) The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? Trends Cogn Sci 4:417–423

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley A (2001) Is working memory working? Am Psychol 56:851–864

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley A (2007) Working memory, thought, and action. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Berger TD, Trinkaus E (1995) Patterns of trauma among the Neandertals. J Archaeol Sci 22:841–852

    Google Scholar 

  • Berna F, Goldberg P, Horwitz LK, Brink J, Holt S, Bamford M, Chazan M (2012) Microstratigraphic evidence of in situ fire in the Acheulean strata of Wonderwerk Cave, Northern Cape province, South Africa. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 109:E1215–E1220

    Google Scholar 

  • Bleed P (1986) The optimal design of hunting weapons: maintainability or reliability? Am Antiq 51:737–747

    Google Scholar 

  • Botha R (2008) Prehistoric shell beads as a window on language evolution. Lang Commun 28:197–212

    Google Scholar 

  • Botha R (2012) Inferring modern language from ancient objects. In: Tallerman M, Gibson KR (eds) The Oxford handbook of language evolution. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 312–393

    Google Scholar 

  • Bramble DM, Lieberman DE (2004) Endurance running and the evolution of Homo. Nature 432:345–352

    Google Scholar 

  • Braun DR, Plummer T, Ditchfield P, Ferraro JV, Maina D, Bishop LC et al (2008) Oldowan behavior and raw material transport: perspectives from the Kanjera Formation. J Archaeol Sci 35:2329–2345

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner E (2003) Fossil traces of the human thought: paleoneurology and the evolution of the genus Homo. Rivista di Antropologia, J Anthropol Sci 81:29–56

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner E (2004) Geometric morphometrics and paleoneurology: brain shape evolution in the genus Homo. J Hum Evol 47:279–303

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner E (2010) Morphological differences in the parietal lobes within the human genus: a neurofunctional perspective. Curr Anthropol 51:S77–S88

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner E, Holloway RL (2010) A bivariate approach to the widening of the frontal lobes in the genus Homo. J Hum Evol 58:138–146

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner E, de la Cuétara JM, Holloway RL (2011) A bivariate approach to the variation of the parietal curvature in the genus Homo. Anat Rec 294:1548–1556

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner E, Jacobs HI (2012) Alzheimer’s disease: the downside of a highly evolved parietal lobe? J Alzheimers Dis 35:227–240

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner E, De Lázaro GR, De la Cuétara JM, Martín-Loeches M, Colom R et al. (2014) Midsagittal brain variation and MRI shape analysis of the precuneus in adult individuals. J Anat 224:1–10

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner E, Manzi G, Arsuaga JL (2003) Encephalization and allometric trajectories in the genus Homo: evidence from the Neandertal and modern lineages. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100:15335–15340

    Google Scholar 

  • Cachel S, Harris JWK (1998) The lifeways of Homo erectus inferred from archaeology and evolutionary ecology: a perspective from East Africa. In: Petraglia MD, Korisettar R (eds) Early human behavior in global context: the rise and diversity of the Lower Palaeolithic record. Routledge, London, pp 108–132

    Google Scholar 

  • Cairney SA, Durrant SJ, Hulleman J, Lewis PA (2014) Targeted memory reactivation during slow wave sleep facilitates emotional memory consolidation. Sleep 37:701–707

    Google Scholar 

  • Caruana MV, d’Errico F, Backwell L (2013) Early hominin social learning strategies underlying the use and production of bone and stone tools. In: Sanz CM, Call J, Boesch C (eds) Tool use in animals: cognition and ecology. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 242–285

    Google Scholar 

  • Cavanna AE, Trimble MR (2006) The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioral correlates. Brain 129:564–583

    Google Scholar 

  • Chiappe DL, Chiappe P (2007) The role of working memory in metaphor production and comprehension. J Mem Lang 56:172–188

    Google Scholar 

  • Churchill SE (2001) Hand morphology, manipulation, and tool use in Neandertals and early modern humans of the Near East. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98:2953–2955

    Google Scholar 

  • Clayton NS, Bussey TJ, Dickinson A (2003) Can animals recall the past and plan for the future? Nat Rev Neurosci 4:685–691

    Google Scholar 

  • Collette F, Van der Linden M (2002) Brain imaging of the central executive component of working memory. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 26:102–125

    Google Scholar 

  • Conneller C (2011) An archaeology of materials: substantial transformations in early prehistoric Europe. Routledge, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Coolidge FL, Overmann KA (2012) Numerosity, abstraction, and the emergence of symbolic thinking. Curr Anthropol 53:204–225

    Google Scholar 

  • Coolidge FL, Wynn T (2001) Executive functions of the frontal lobes and the evolutionary ascendancy of Homo sapiens. Camb Archaeol J 11:255–260

    Google Scholar 

  • Coolidge FL, Wynn T (2005) Working memory, its executive functions, and the emergence of modern thinking. Camb Archaeol J 15:5–26

    Google Scholar 

  • Coolidge FL (2007) The working memory account of Neandertal cognition—How phonological storage capacity may be related to recursion and the pragmatics of modern speech. J Hum Evol 52:707–710

    Google Scholar 

  • Coolidge FL, Wynn T (2009) The rise of Homo sapiens: the evolution of modern thinking. Wiley, Hoboken

    Google Scholar 

  • Coolidge FL, Wynn T (2012) Cognitive prerequisites for a language of diplomacy. In: Tallerman M, Gibson KR (eds) The oxford handbook of language evolution. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 216–223

    Google Scholar 

  • Coolidge FL, Overmann KA, Wynn T (2011) Recursion: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci 2:547–554

    Google Scholar 

  • Culham JC, Kanwisher NG (2001) Neuroimaging of cognitive functions in human parietal cortex. Cogn Neurosci 11:157–163

    Google Scholar 

  • d’Errico F, Henshilwood C, Vanhaeren M, Van Niekerk K (2005) Nassarius kraussianus shell beads from Blombos Cave: evidence for symbolic behaviour in the middle stone age. J Hum Evol 48:3–24

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson I, Noble W (1989) The archaeology of perception: traces of depiction and language. Curr Anthropol 30:125–155

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson I, Noble W (1993) Tools and language in human evolution. In: Gibson K, Ingold T (eds) Tools, language, and cognition in human evolution. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 263–288

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene S (2011) The number sense, 2nd edn. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene S, Brannon EM (Eds) (2011) Space, time, and number: searching for the foundations of mathematical thought. Academic Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • De Smet HJ, Paquier P, Verhoeven J, Mariën P (2013) The cerebellum: its role in language and related cognitive and affective functions. Brain Lang 127:334–342

    Google Scholar 

  • Domínguez-Rodrigo M, Pickering TR, Bunn HT (2010) Configurational approach to identifying the earliest hominin butchers. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 107:20929–20934

    Google Scholar 

  • Donald M (1991) Origins of the modern mind: three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar RIM (2003) The social brain: mind, language, and society in evolutionary perspective. Annu Rev Anthropol 32:163–181

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernández-Laso MC, Navarro MGC, García-Antón MD, Rivals F (2011) Territorial mobility of Neanderthal groups: a case study from level M of Abric Romaní (Capellades, Barcelona, Spain). In: Conard NJ, Richter J (eds) Neanderthal lifeways, subsistence and technology: 150 years of Neanderthal study. Springer, Dordrecht, pp 187–202

    Google Scholar 

  • Franklin MS, Zyphur MJ (2005) The role of dreams in the evolution of the human mind. Evol Psychol 3:59–78

    Google Scholar 

  • Fruth B, Hohmann G (1996) Nest building behavior in the great apes: the great leap forward? In: McGrew WC, Marchant LF, Nishida T (eds) Great ape societies. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 225–239

    Google Scholar 

  • Gais S, Plihal W, Wagner U, Born J (2000) Early sleep triggers memory for early visual discrimination skills. Nat Neurosci 3:1335–1339

    Google Scholar 

  • García-Antón MD, Granda LM, Navarro MGC (2011) Level G of Las Fuentes de San Cristóbal (Southern Pyrenees, Spain): availability of lithic resources and territory management. In: Conard NJ, Richter J (eds) Neanderthal lifeways, subsistence and technology: 150 years of Neanderthal Study. Springer, Dordrecht, pp 203–219

    Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaniga M, Ivry R, Mangun G (2013) Cognitive neuroscience, 4th edn. W.W. Norton & Co, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Geva S, Jones PS, Crinion JT, Price CJ, Baron J-C, Warburton EA (2011) The neural correlates of inner speech defined by voxel-based lesion symptom mapping. Brain 134:3071–3082

    Google Scholar 

  • Graves RR, Lupo AC, McCarthy RC, Wescott DJ, Cunningham DL (2010) Just how strapping was KNM-WT 15000? J Hum Evol 59:542–554

    Google Scholar 

  • Gunz P, Neubauer S, Maureille B, Hublin JJ (2010) Brain development after birth differs between Neanderthals and modern humans. Curr Biol 20:R921–R922

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartmann E (1998) Dreams and nightmares: the new theory on the origin and meaning of dreams. Plenum Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser MD, Chomsky N, Fitch WT (2002) The faculty of language: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science 298:1569–1579

    Google Scholar 

  • Hazy T, Frank M, O’Reilly R (2006) Banishing the homunculus: making working memory work. Neuroscience 139:105–118

    Google Scholar 

  • Henshilwood C, Dubreuil B (2009) Reading the artefacts: Gleaning language skills from the Middle Stone Age in southern Africa. In: Botha R (ed) The cradle of language, Stellenbosch, South Africa, pp 41–63

    Google Scholar 

  • Henshilwood CS, d’Errico F, Vanhaeren M, van Niekerk K, Jacobs Z (2004) Middle Stone Age shell beads from South Africa. Science 304:404

    Google Scholar 

  • Hodgson D (2009) Evolution of the visual cortex and the emergence of symmetry in the Acheulean techno-complex. CR Palevol 8:93–97

    Google Scholar 

  • Holloway RL (1996) Toward a synthetic theory of human brain evolution. In: Changeaux JP, Chavaillon J (eds) Origins of the human brain. Clarendon Press, Oxford, pp 42–54

    Google Scholar 

  • Kane M, Engle R (2002) The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: an individual-differences perspective. Psychon Bull Rev 9:637–671

    Google Scholar 

  • Karni A, Tanne D, Rubenstein BS, Askenasy JJ, Sagi D (1994) Dependence on REM sleep of overnight improvement of a perceptual skill. Science 265:679–682

    Google Scholar 

  • Keller C, Keller J (1996) Cognition and tool use: the blacksmith at work. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein R (2000) Archeology and the evolution of human behavior. Evol Anthropol 9:17–36

    Google Scholar 

  • Koenigs M, Barbey AK, Postle BR, Grafman J (2009) Superior parietal cortex is critical for the manipulation of information in working memory. J Neurosci 29:14980–14986

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohn M, Mithen S (1999) Handaxes: products of sexual selection? Antiquity 73:518–526

    Google Scholar 

  • Koziol LF, Budding DE, Chidekel D (2010) Adaptation, expertise, and giftedness: towards an understanding of cortical, subcortical, and cerebellar network contributions. Cerebellum 9:499–529

    Google Scholar 

  • Krippner S, Hughes W (1970) Genius at work. Psychol Today June 40–43

    Google Scholar 

  • Land MF (2014) Do we have an internal model of the outside world? Philos Trans R Soc B 369:20130045

    Google Scholar 

  • Lezak M (1982) The problem of assessing executive functions. Int J Psychol 17:281–297

    Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman DE, Bramble DM, Raichlen DA, Shea JJ (2009) Brains, brawn and the evolution of human endurance running capabilities. In: Grine FE, Fleagle JG, Leakey RE (eds) The first humans: origin and early evolution of the genus Homo. Springer, New York pp 77–98

    Google Scholar 

  • Lombard M, Haidle M (2012) Thinking a bow-and-arrow set: cognitive implications of Middle Stone Age bow and stone-tipped arrow technology. Camb Archaeol J 22:237–264

    Google Scholar 

  • Lou HC, Luber B, Crupain M, Keenan JP, Nowak M, Kjaer TW et al (2004) Parietal cortex and representation of the mental self. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101:6827–6832

    Google Scholar 

  • Malafouris L (2008) Beads for a plastic mind: the ‘Blind Man’s Stick’ (BMS) hypothesis and the active nature of material culture. Camb Archaeol J 18:401–414

    Google Scholar 

  • Malafouris L (2010) Grasping the concept of number: how did the sapient mind move beyond approximation? In: Renfrew C, Morley I (eds) The archaeology of measurement: comprehending heaven, earth and time in ancient societies. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 35–42

    Google Scholar 

  • Malafouris L (2013) How things shape the mind: A theory of material engagement. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Malcolm-Smith S, Solms M (2004) Incidence of threat in dreams: a response to Revonsuo’s threat simulation theory. Dreaming 14:220–229

    Google Scholar 

  • Marean C (2010) Introduction to the special issue: the Middle Stone Age at Pinnacle Point Site 13B, a coastal cave near Mossel Bay (Western Cape Province, South Africa). J Hum Evol 59:231–233

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin-Ordas G, Call J (2013) Episodic memory: a comparative approach. Front Behav Neurosci 7:1–13

    Google Scholar 

  • McGrew WC (1992) Chimpanzee material culture. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • McGrew WC (2004) The cultured chimpanzee: reflections on cultural primatology. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • McPherron SP (2000) Handaxes as a measure of the mental abilities of early hominids. J Archaeol Sci 27:655–663

    Google Scholar 

  • McPherron SP, Alemseged Z, Marean CW, Wynn JG, Reed D, Geraads D et al. (2010) Evidence for stone-tool-assisted consumption of animal tissues before 3.39 million years ago at Dikika, Ethiopia. Nature 466:857–860

    Google Scholar 

  • Mithen S (1999) Imitation and cultural change: a view from the Stone Age, with specific reference to the manufacture of handaxes. In: Box HO, Gibson KR (eds) Mammalian social learning: comparative and ecological perspectives. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 389–399

    Google Scholar 

  • Miyake A, Shah P (1999) Models of working memory: mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Neubauer S, Gunz P, Hublin JJ (2009) The pattern of endocranial ontogenetic shape changes in humans. J Anat 215:240–255

    Google Scholar 

  • Neubauer S, Gunz P, Hublin JJ (2010) Endocranial shape changes during growth in chimpanzees and humans: a morphometric analysis of unique and shared aspects. J Hum Evol 59:555–566

    Google Scholar 

  • Noble W, Davidson I (1996) Human evolution, language, and mind: a psychological and archaeological inquiry. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Connell JF, Allen J (2004) Dating the colonization of Sahul (Pleistocene Australia-New Guinea): a review of recent research. J Archaeol Sci 31:835–853

    Google Scholar 

  • Ogawa A, Yamazaki Y, Ueno K, Cheng K, Iriki A (2010) Inferential reasoning by exclusion recruits parietal and prefrontal cortices. Neuroimage 52:1603–1610

    Google Scholar 

  • Ohayon MM, Carskadon MA, Guilleminault C, Vitiello MV (2004) Meta-analysis of quantitative sleep parameters from childhood to old age in healthy individuals: developing normative sleep values across the human lifespan. Sleep 27:1255–127

    Google Scholar 

  • Orban GA (2011) The extraction of 3D shape in the visual system of human and nonhuman primates. Annu Rev Neurosci 34:361–388

    Google Scholar 

  • Orban GA, Caruana F (2014) The neural basis of human tool use. Front Psychol 5:1–12

    Google Scholar 

  • Osaka N, Logie R, d’Esposito M (eds) (2007) The cognitive neuroscience of working memory. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Oswalt W (1976) An anthropological analysis of food-getting technology. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Overmann KA (2014) Finger-counting in the Upper Paleolithic. Rock Art Research 31:63–80

    Google Scholar 

  • Peeters RR, Simone L, Nelissen K, Fabbri-Destro M, Vanduffel W, Rizzolatti G et al (2009) The representation of tool use in humans and monkeys: common and uniquely human features. J Neurosci 29:11523–11539

    Google Scholar 

  • Peeters RR, Rizzolatti G, Orban GA (2013) Functional properties of the left parietal tool use region. Neuroimage 78:83–93

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramachandran VS (2004) A brief tour of human consciousness: from impostor poodles to purple numbers. Pi Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Reuland E (2005) On the evolution and genesis of language: the force of imagination. Lingue e Linguaggio 4:81–110

    Google Scholar 

  • Revonsuo A, Valli K (2000) Dreaming and consciousness: testing the threat simulation theory of the function of dreaming. Psyche 6 [Internet]. 2000 [cited 2014 January 11]. http://www.theassc.org/files/assc/2467.pdf

  • Revonsuo A (2000) The reinterpretation of dreams: an evolutionary hypothesis of the function of dreaming. Behav Brain Sci 23:793–1121

    Google Scholar 

  • Roebroeks W, Kolen J, Rensink E (1988) Planning depth, anticipation and the organization of Middle Palaeolithic technology: “Archaic natives” meet Eve’s descendents. Helenium 28:17–34

    Google Scholar 

  • Rouillon A (2006) Au Gravettien, dans la grotte Cosquer (Marseille, Bouches-du-Rhône), l’Homme a-t-il compté sur ses doigts? L’anthropologie 110:500–509

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruff CB (1991) Climate and body shape in hominid evolution. J Hum Evol 21:81–104

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruff CB (1993) Climatic adaptation and hominid evolution: the thermoregulatory imperative. Evol Anthropol 2:53–60

    Google Scholar 

  • Sabater Pi J, Veá JJ, Serrallonga J (1997) Did the first hominids build nests? Curr Anthropol 38:914–916

    Google Scholar 

  • Savage-Rumbaugh ES, Toth N, Schick K (2007) Kanzi learns to knap stone tools. In: Washburn DA (ed) Primate perspectives on behavior and cognition. American Psychological Association, Washington, pp 279–291

    Google Scholar 

  • Schick K, Toth N (2006) An overview of the Oldowan industrial complex: the sites and the nature of their evidence. In: Toth N, Schick K (eds) The Oldowan: case studies into the earliest stone age. Stone Age Institute Press, Gosport, pp 3–42

    Google Scholar 

  • Schick K, Toth N (2009) The cutting edge: new approaches to the archaeology of human origins. Stone Age Institute Press, Gosport

    Google Scholar 

  • Schlanger N (1996) Understanding Levallois: lithic technology and cognitive archaeology. Camb Archaeol J 6:231–254

    Google Scholar 

  • Segal DL, Coolidge FL, Rosowsky E (2006) Personality disorders and older adults: diagnosis, assessment, and treatment. John Wiley & Sons, Hoboken

    Google Scholar 

  • Seghier ML (2012) The angular gyrus: multiple functions and multiple subdivisions. Neuroscientist 19:1–19

    Google Scholar 

  • Semaw S, Renne P, Harris JWK, Feibel CS, Bernor RL, Fesseha N, Mowbray K (1997) 2.5-million-year-old stone tools from Gona Ethiopia. Nature 385:333–336

    Google Scholar 

  • Semaw S, Rogers MJ, Quade J, Renne PR, Butler RF, Dominguez-Rodrigo M, Simpson SW (2003) 2.6-million-year-old stone tools and associated bones from OGS-6 and OGS-7, Gona, Afar, Ethiopia. J Hum Evol 45:169–177

    Google Scholar 

  • Semaw S, Rogers M, Stout D (2009) Insights into late Pliocene lithic assemblage variability: the East Gona and Ounda Gona South Oldowan archaeology (2.6 million years ago), Afar, Ethiopia. In: Schick K, Toth N (eds) The cutting edge: new approaches to the archaeology of human origins. Stone Age Institute Press, Gosport, pp 211–246

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherratt A (1997) Climatic cycles and behavioural revolutions: the emergence of modern humans and the beginning of farming. Antiquity 71:187–271

    Google Scholar 

  • Spinapolice EE (2012) Raw material economy in Salento (Apulia, Italy): new perspectives on Neanderthal mobility patterns. J Archaeol Sci 39:680–689

    Google Scholar 

  • Spoor F, Zonneveld F (1998) Comparative review of the human bony labyrinth Yearb Phys Anthropol 41:211–251

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanley SM (1992) An ecological theory for the origin of Homo. Paleobiology 18:237–257

    Google Scholar 

  • Stickgold R, James L, Hobson JA (2000) Visual discrimination learning requires sleep after training. Nat Neurosci 2:1237–1238

    Google Scholar 

  • Steudel-Numbers K (2006) Energetics in Homo erectus and other early hominins: the consequences of increased lower limb length. J Hum Evol 51:445–453

    Google Scholar 

  • Stout D (2002) Skill and cognition in stone tool production. Curr Anthropol 43:693–722

    Google Scholar 

  • Stout D, Chaminade T (2007) The evolutionary neuroscience of tool making. Neuropsychologia 45:1091–1100

    Google Scholar 

  • Stout D, Passingham R, Frith C, Apel J, Chaminade T (2011) Technology, expertise, and social cognition in human evolution. Eur J Neurosci 33:1328–1338

    Google Scholar 

  • Stout D, Toth N, Schick K, Chaminade T (2008) Neural correlates of Early Stone Age toolmaking: technology, language, and cognition in human cognition. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 363:1939–1949

    Google Scholar 

  • Stout D, Toth N, Schick K, Stout J, Hutchins G (2000) Stone tool-making and brain activation: positron emission tomography (PET) studies. J Archaeol Sci 27:1215–1223

    Google Scholar 

  • Suddendorf T, Corballis MC (2007) The evolution of foresight: what is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? Behav Brain Sci 30:299–351

    Google Scholar 

  • Tononi G, Cirelli C (2014) Sleep and the price of plasticity: from synaptic and cellular homeostasis to memory consolidation and integration. Neuron 81:12–34

    Google Scholar 

  • Toth N (1985) The Oldowan reassesed: a closer look at early stone artifacts. J Archaeol Sci 12:101–120

    Google Scholar 

  • Toth N (1987) Behavioral inferences from Early Stone Age artifact assemblages: an experimental model. J Hum Evol 16:763–787

    Google Scholar 

  • Toth N, Schick K (2009) The Oldowan: the tool making of early hominins and chimpanzees compared. Annu Rev Anthropol 38:289–305

    Google Scholar 

  • Toth N, Schick K (eds) (2006) The Oldowan: case studies into the earliest stone age. Stone Age Institute Press, Gosport

    Google Scholar 

  • Trinkaus E (1995) Neanderthal mortality patterns. J Archaeol Sci 22:121–142

    Google Scholar 

  • Trinkaus E (2012) Neandertals, early modern humans, and rodeo riders. J Archaeol Sci 39:3691–3693

    Google Scholar 

  • Tulving E (2002) Episodic memory: from mind to brain. Annu Rev Psychol 53:1–25

    Google Scholar 

  • Twomey T (2013) The cognitive implications of controlled use of fire. Camb Archaeol J 23:113–128

    Google Scholar 

  • Valli K, Revonsuo A, Pälkäs O, Ismahil KH, Ali KJ, Punamäki R-L (2005) The threat simulation theory of the evolutionary function of dreaming: evidence from dreams of traumatized children. Conscious Cogn 14:188–218

    Google Scholar 

  • Van de Castle RL (1983) Animal figures in fantasy and dreams. In: Kacher A, Beck A (eds) New perspectives on our lives with companion animals. University of Pennsylvania Press, Philadelphia, pp 148–173

    Google Scholar 

  • Vandervert LR (2003a) How working memory and cognitive modeling functions of the cerebellum contribute to discoveries in mathematics. New Ideas Psychol 21:159–175

    Google Scholar 

  • Vandervert LR (2003b) The neurophysiological basis of innovation. In: Shavinina LV (ed) The international handbook on innovation. Elsevier Science, Oxford, pp 17–30

    Google Scholar 

  • Vandervert LR (2009) The appearance of the child prodigy 10,000 years ago: an evolutionary and developmental explanation. J Mind Behav 30:15–32

    Google Scholar 

  • Vandervert LR (2011) The evolution of language: the cerebro-cerebellar blending of visual-spatial working memory with vocalizations. J Mind Behav 32:317–332

    Google Scholar 

  • Vandervert LR, Schimpf PH, Liu H (2007) How working memory and the cerebellum collaborate to produce creativity and innovation. Creat Res J 19:1–18

    Google Scholar 

  • Vanhaeren M, d’Errico F (2006) Aurignacian ethno-linguistic geography of Europe revealed by personal ornaments. J Archaeol Sci 33:1105–1128

    Google Scholar 

  • Vanhaeren M, d’Errico F, Van Niekerk K, Henshilwood C, Erasmus R (2013) Thinking strings: additional evidence for personal ornament use in the Middle Stone Age at Blombos Cave, South Africa. J Hum Evol 64:500–517

    Google Scholar 

  • Wadley L (2010) Were snares and traps used in the Middle Stone Age and does it matter? a review and a case study from Sibudu, South Africa. J Hum Evol 58:179–192

    Google Scholar 

  • Wadley L, Hodgskiss T, Grant M (2009) Implications for complex cognition from the hafting of tools with compound adhesives in the Middle Stone Age, South Africa. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106:9590–9594

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner U, Gais S, Haider H, Verleger R, Born J (2004) Sleep inspires insight. Nature 427:352–355

    Google Scholar 

  • Weaver AH (2005) Reciprocal evolution of the cerebellum and neocortex in fossil humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102:3576–3580

    Google Scholar 

  • Whiten A, Byrne R (1991) The emergence of metarepresentation in human ontogeny and primate phylogeny. In: Whiten A (ed) Natural theories of mind. Blackwell, Oxford, pp 267–281

    Google Scholar 

  • Whiten A, Ham R (1992) On the nature and evolution of imitation in the animal kingdom: reappraisal of a century of research. In: Slater P, Rosenblatt J, Beer J, Milinski M (ed) Advances in the study of behavior, Academic Press, New York, vol 21, pp 239–283

    Google Scholar 

  • Winson J (1990) The meaning of dreams. Sci Am 263:89–96

    Google Scholar 

  • Wong K (2006) Lucy’s baby. Sci Am 295:78–85

    Google Scholar 

  • Woods AD (2011) The effects of lithic raw material quality on Aurignacian blade production at Abri Cellier. Doctoral dissertation, University of Iowa. http://ir.uiowa.edu/etd/1111

  • Wynn T (1981) The intelligence of Oldowan hominids. J Hum Evol 10:529–541

    Google Scholar 

  • Wynn T (1995) Handaxe enigmas. World Archaeol 27:10–24

    Google Scholar 

  • Wynn T (2002) Archaeology and cognitive evolution. Behav Brain Sci 25:389–438

    Google Scholar 

  • Wynn T (2009a) Hafted spears and the archaeology of mind. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106:9544–9545

    Google Scholar 

  • Wynn T (2009b) Whither evolutionary cognitive archaeology? Afterword. In: de Beaune S, Coolidge FL, Wynn T (eds) Cognitive archaeology and human evolution. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 145–149

    Google Scholar 

  • Wynn T, Coolidge FL (2003) The role of working memory in the evolution of managed foraging. Before Farming 2:1–16

    Google Scholar 

  • Wynn T, Coolidge FL (2010) Beyond symbolism and language: an introduction to supplement 1, working memory. Curr Anthropol 51(Supplement1):S5–S16

    Google Scholar 

  • Wynn T, Coolidge FL (2011) The implications of the working memory model for the evolution of modern cognition. Int J Evol Biol [Internet]. 2011 [cited 2014 January 11]; Article ID 741357, p 12. doi:10.4061/2011/741357

  • Wynn T, Coolidge FL (2012) How to think like a Neandertal. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Wynn T, Coolidge F, Bright M (2009) Hohlenstein-Stadel and the evolution of human conceptual thought. Camb Archaeol J 19(1):73–83

    Google Scholar 

  • Wynn T, Hernandez-Aguilar RA, Marchant L, McGrew WC (2011) “An ape’s view of the Oldowan” revisited. Evol Anthropol 20:181–197

    Google Scholar 

  • Zilhão J, Angelucci DE, Badal-Garcia E, d’Errico F, Daniel F, Dayet L, Zapata J (2010) Symbolic use of marine shells and mineral pigments by Iberian Neandertals. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 107:1023–1028

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Frederick L. Coolidge .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Coolidge, F.L., Wynn, T., Overmann, K.A., Hicks, J.M. (2015). Cognitive Archaeology and the Cognitive Sciences. In: Bruner, E. (eds) Human Paleoneurology. Springer Series in Bio-/Neuroinformatics, vol 3. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-08500-5_8

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-08500-5_8

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-08499-2

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-08500-5

  • eBook Packages: EngineeringEngineering (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics