Skip to main content

The Naturalistic Tradition

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Distant Connections: The Memory Basis of Creative Analogy

Part of the book series: SpringerBriefs in Psychology ((BRIEFSCOGNIT))

  • 179 Accesses

Abstract

Traditional experimental studies of analogical retrieval converged in documenting the centrality of superficial similarities during the retrieval of analogous situations from long-term memory. The present Chapter reviews a more recent generation of observational studies which began to analyze the spontaneous analogies produced by professionals of several disciplines as they work in their areas of expertise. The analogies generated within scientific domains such as Biology, Psychology, or Geology revealed a close thematic connection between the target analogs and the retrieved sources, with distant analogizing being mostly restricted to the goal of illustrating ideas to others. However, the productive analogies generated by journalists, economists, educators, and developers of industrial products were drawn to the same domain as the target in a small proportion of the cases, thus suggesting that the retrieval of analogous cases is not invariably constrained by superficial similarity. After reviewing the use of analogies within this varied array of professional activities, we succinctly describe the computer models that were engineered to simulate this pattern of analogical retrieval.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Bearman, C. R., Ball, L. J., & Ormerod, T. C. (2007). The structure and function of spontaneous analogising in domain-based problem solving. Thinking and Reasoning, 13, 273–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blanchette, I., & Dunbar, K. (2001). Analogy use in naturalistic settings: The influence of audience, emotion, and goals. Memory & Cognition, 29, 730–735.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brunswick, E. (1943). Organismic achievement and environmental probability. Psychological Review, 50, 255–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chan, J., Paletz, S. B., & Schunn, C. D. (2012). Analogy as a strategy for supporting complex problem solving under uncertainty. Memory & Cognition, 40, 1352–1365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christensen, B. T., & Schunn, C. D. (2007). The relationship of analogical distance to analogical function and pre-inventive structure: The case of engineering design. Memory & Cognition, 35, 29–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, K. (1997). How scientists think: Online creativity and conceptual change in science. In T. B. Ward, S. M. Smith, & S. Vaid (Eds.), Creative thought: An investigation on conceptual structures and processes (pp. 461–493). Washington DC: APA Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., & Simon, H. A. (1993). Protocol analysis: Verbal reports as data (2nd ed.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D., Rattermann, M. J., & Forbus, K. D. (1993). The roles of similarity in transfer: Separating retrievability from inferential soundness. Cognitive Psychology, 25, 431–467.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holyoak, K. J., & Thagard, P. R. (1995). Mental leaps: Analogy in creative thought. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Khong, Y. F. (1992). Analogies at war: Korea, Munich, Dien Bien Phu, and the Vietnam decisions of 1965. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Klahr, D. (1999). Exploring science: The cognition and development of discovery processes. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kretz, D. R., & Krawczyk, D. C. (2014). Expert analogy use in a naturalistic setting. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lange, T. & Wharton, C. (1993). Dynamic memories: Analysis of an integrated comprehension and episodic memory retrieval model. In Proceedings of the thirteenth international conference on artificial intelligence (pp. 208–213).

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewin, K. (1943). Defining the field at a given time. Psychological Review, 50, 292–310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nersessian, N. J. (1992). How do scientists think? Capturing the dynamics of conceptual change in science. In R. N. Giere (Ed.), Cognitive models of science (pp. 5–22). Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Keefe, D., & Costello, F. (2008). A fast computational model of analogical retrieval (and mapping). In B. C. Love, K. McRae, & V. M. Sloutsky (Eds.), Proceedings of the 30th annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2003–2008). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olguín, V., Trench, M., & Minervino, R. (2017). Attending to individual recipients’ knowledge when Generating persuasive analogies. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 29, 755–768.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Owens, C. (1993). Integrating feature extraction and memory search. Machine Learning, 10, 311–339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richland, L. E., Holyoak, K. J., & Stigler, J. W. (2004). Analogy use in eighth-grade mathematics classrooms. Cognition and Instruction, 22, 37–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saner, L., & Schunn, C. D. (1999). Analogies out of the blue: When history seems to retell itself. In M. Hahn & S. Stoness (Eds.), Proceedings of the 21st annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 619–624). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schank, R. C. (1982). Dynamic memory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schank, R. C., & Abelson, R. P. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals and understanding: An inquiry into human knowledge structures. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stigler, J. W., Gallimore, R., & Hiebert, J. (2000). Using video surveys to compare classrooms and teaching across cultures: Examples and lessons from the TIMSS video studies. Educational Psychologist, 35, 87–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stigler, J. W., Gonzales, P., Kawanaka, T., Knoll, S., & Serrano, A. (1999). The TIMSS videotape class-room study: Methods and findings from an exploratory research project on eighth-grade mathematics instruction in Germany, Japan, and the United States. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thagard, P. R. (1999). How scientists explain disease. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wertheimer, M. (1945). Productive thinking. New York: Harper.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2020 The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Trench, M., Minervino, R.A. (2020). The Naturalistic Tradition. In: Distant Connections: The Memory Basis of Creative Analogy. SpringerBriefs in Psychology(). Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-52545-3_3

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics