Skip to main content

When Nobody Wins

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Competition in Inflection and Word-Formation

Part of the book series: Studies in Morphology ((SUMO,volume 5))

Abstract

We argue that lexical gaps arise when language learners fail to find productive rules in a morphological domain. Using the Tolerance Principle as a formal model of productivity, we show that lexical gaps can be predicted on purely numerical grounds using lexical statistics, with case studies on Spanish, Polish, and Russian. The learnability approach taken here leads to simpler theories of morphology.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 149.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. 1.

    This chapter adapts and expands upon analyses first presented in chapter 5 of Yang (2016). We thank Margaret Borowczyk for assistance with Polish, and Jennifer Preys and Vitaly Nikolaev for assistance with Russian. We are also grateful for the comments and suggestions provided by Wolfgang Dressler, Gregory Stump, and the editors of the present volume.

  2. 2.

    For a quantitative analysis of the *stridden gap and the failings of frequency and other measures of indirect negative evidence as a solution to the gap problem, see Yang (2016), section 5.1.1.

  3. 3.

    We avoid the competing term paradigm (or paradigmatic) gap, as we do not wish to make a commitment to the theoretical notion of the inflectional paradigm.

  4. 4.

    Orgun and Sprouse (1999) propose a different, albeit roughly equivalent, mechanism for encoding defectivity in Optimality Theory; see Fanselow and Féry (2002) and Rice and Blaho (2009) for discussion of these mechanisms.

  5. 5.

    See also chapter 5 of Yang (2016) for criticisms of a more subtle, frequency-based, form of lexical conservatism that uses indirect negative evidence.

  6. 6.

    We do not claim that the language user will activate the full lexical entries for the irregulars before accessing the regulars. It is conceivable that the computational search process deals with a hashed list, as described in (1): the entries are “keys” indexed by the semantic and phonological properties of words, which point to full lexical entries in long-term memory, rather than lexical entries themselves.

  7. 7.

    Baayen’s formulation unfortunately, and tautologically, quantifies productivity using an inherently continuous measure, a ratio.

  8. 8.

    Albright et al. (2001) argue that there is some weak segmental conditioning on the application of diphthongization. Even if this is correct—and the evidence is not overwhelming—it does not obviate the need for lexical diacritics.

  9. 9.

    These e-i alternations are also present in deverbal derivatives (Harris 1969, 110; Lerner 2011).

  10. 10.

    This correctly predicts that this pattern will be restricted to the third conjugation, since only this conjugation has an -i- theme vowel needed to condition the lowering rule.

  11. 11.

    Note that the g-j alternation in the conjugation of sumergir is purely orthographic; both are [x] throughout.

  12. 12.

    Many Spanish verb stems have multiple prefixal variants; for instance, desvestir ‘to undress’, investir ‘to invest’, revestir ‘to decorate’, and tra(s)vestir(se) ‘to cross-dress’ are all plausibly derived from vestir ‘to dress’. Without exception, all verbs derived from the same stem undergo the same set of stem changes, and therefore we collect counts over verb stems rather than verbs. Verbs were manually grouped by stem, with an etymological dictionary (Roberts 2014) used to adjudicate unclear cases.

  13. 13.

    We analyze the three conjugations separately because it has long been theorized that the productivity of the various stem changes may be a function of conjugation. Note, however, that the Tolerance predictions are the same if the first two conjugations are grouped together.

  14. 14.

    Whereas in Table 3 we collapse prefixal variants of a stem into a single entry, Albright does not mention any such practice, so inputs to the MGL system in this experiment are verb stems plus any prefix(es), rather than stems.

  15. 15.

    However, it may still be the case that subjective frequencies used by Albright are better predictors of defectivity than the corpus-based frequency norms used here.

  16. 16.

    This calculation is complicated slightly by a small number of masculine nouns which take -a in the gen.sg. and -u in the dative singular. Since this word list does not provide a way to determine the case of individual tokens, the count of -u nouns (516) may be a slight overestimate, but it is unlikely this would change the results, as the number of nouns potentially affected is rather small and both classes are quite far from the productivity threshold.

  17. 17.

    The only clear exception is the labial stem zatmit’ ‘to eclipse’. Moskvin (2015, cited in Pertsova 2016) argues this gap is due to phonotactic illformedness.

  18. 18.

    Pertsova (2016) distinguishes between t-stems and st-stems, presumably because the s of the latter is never present in the 1sg. non-past; e.g., vyrastit’-vyrašču ‘raise, cultivate’, but *vyra[stʃj]u. However, this can be handled with an additional—surface-true—phonological rule simplifying the resulting sibilant cluster, as [stʃj] is not a valid onset in Russian (Vitaly Nikolaev, p.c.).

  19. 19.

    As in Spanish, many Russian verbs are derived from prefixation to stems, but with rare exceptions, all derivatives of a stem undergo the same mutation in the 1sg. non-past. Therefore, Pertsova (2016) collects counts over stems rather than full verbs.

  20. 20.

    Pertsova (ibid.) counts 38 non-defective sš stems and 36 non-defective zž stems. Assuming these are related by a single rule of retroflexion, as seems likely, they can tolerate no more than 20 non-alternating exceptions.

References

  • Albright, Adam. 2003. A Quantitative Study of Spanish Paradigm Gaps. In Proceedings of the 22th West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics, 1–14. Somerville: Cascadilla.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2009. Lexical and Morphological Conditioning of Paradigm Gaps. In Modeling Ungrammaticality in Optimality Theory, ed. Sylvia Blaho and Curt Rice, 117–164. London: Equinox.

    Google Scholar 

  • Albright, Adam, and Bruce Hayes. 2003. Rules vs. Analogy in English Past Tenses: A Computational/Experimental Study. Cognition 90: 119–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Albright, Adam, Argelia Andrade, and Bruce Hayes. 2001. Segmental Environments of Spanish Diphthongization. In UCLA Working Papers in Linguistics 7: Papers in Phonology 5, 117–151. Los Angeles: University of California.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, Stephen R. 1969. West Scandinavian Vowel Systems and the Ordering of Phonological Rules. Doctoral dissertation, MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2010. Failing One’s Obligations: Defectiveness in Rumantsch Reflexes of DĒBĒRE. In Defective Paradigms: Missing Forms and What They Tell Us, ed. Matthew Baerman, Greville G. Corbett, and Dunstan Brown, 19–34. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aronoff, Mark. 1976. Word Formation in Generative Grammar. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baayen, R. Harald. 2009. Corpus Linguistics in Morphology: Morphological Productivity. In Corpus Linguistics: An International Handbook, ed. Anke Ludeling and Merja Kyto, 899–919. Berlin/Boston: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baerman, Matthew. 2008. Historical Observations on Defectiveness: The First Singular Non-past. Russian Linguistics 32: 81–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baerman, Matthew, and Greville G. Corbett. 2010. Defectiveness: Typology and Diachrony. In Defective Paradigms: Missing Forms and What They Tell Us, ed. Matthew Baerman, Greville G. Corbett, and Dunstan Brown, 19–34. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Baerman, Matthew, Greville G. Corbett, and Dunstan Brown (eds.). 2010. Defective Paradigms: Missing Forms and What They Tell Us. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baronian, Luc. 2005. North of Phonology. Doctoral dissertation, Stanford University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, Laurie. 2001. Morphological Productivity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Berko, Jean. 1958. The Child’s Learning of English Morphology. Word 14: 150–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyé, Gilles, and Patricia Cabredo Hofherr. 2010. Defectivity as stem suppletion in French and Spanish verbs. In Defective Paradigms: Missing Forms and What They Tell Us, ed. Matthew Baerman, Greville G. Corbett, and Dunstan Brown, 35–52. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brame, Michael K, and Ivonne Bordelois. 1973. Vocalic Alternations in Spanish. Linguistic Inquiry 4: 111–168.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, Roger. 1973. A First Language: The Early Stages. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Butt, John, and Carmen Benjamin. 1988. A New Reference Grammar of Modern Spanish. London: Edward Arnold.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam. 1965. Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2005. Three Factors in Language Design. Linguistic Inquiry 36: 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clahsen, Harald. 1999. Lexical Entries and Rules of Language: A Multidisciplinary Study of German Inflection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22: 991–1069.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clahsen, Harald, Monika Rothweiler, Andreas Woest, and Gary Marcus. 1992. Regular and Irregular Inflection in the Acquisition of German Noun Plurals. Cognition 45: 225–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clahsen, Harald, Fraibet Aveledo, and Iggy Roca. 2002. The Development of Regular and Irregular Verb Inflection in Spanish Child Language. Journal of Child Language 29: 591–622.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • D ąbrowska, Ewa. 2001. Learning a Morphological System Without a Default: The Polish Genitive. Journal of Child Language 28: 545–574.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2005. Productivity and Beyond: Mastering the Polish Genitive Inflection. Journal of Child Language 32: 191–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dressler, Wolfgang U. 1999. Why Collapse Morphological Concepts? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22: 1021.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dressler, Wolfgang U., Lavinia Merlini Barbaresi, Sonja Schwaiger, Jutta Ransmayr, Sabine Sommer-Lolei, and Katharina Korecky-Kröll. 2019. Rivalry and Lack of Blocking Among Italian and German Diminutives in Adult and Child Language. In Competition in Inflection and Word-Formation, ed. Franz Rainer, Francesco Gardani, Wolfgang U. Dressler, and Hans Christian Luschützky, 123–143. Cham: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elsen, Hilke. 2002. The Acquisition of German Plurals. In Morphology 2000: Selected Papers from the 9th Morphology Meeting, Vienna, 25–27 Feb 2000, 117–128. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Fanselow, Gisbert, and Caroline Féry. 2002. Resolving Conflicts in Grammars: Optimality Theory in Syntax, Morphology, and Phonology. Hamburg: Helmut Buske.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, Judith C., Philip S. Dale, and Ping Li. 2008. Does Frequency Count? Parental Input and the Acquisition of Vocabulary. Journal of Child Language 35: 515–531.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hahn, Ulrike, and Ramin C. Nakisa. 2000. German Inflection: Single Route or Dual Route? Cognitive Psychology 41: 313–360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halle, Morris. 1973. Prolegomena to a Theory of Word Formation. Linguistic Inquiry 4: 3–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halle, Morris, and Alec Marantz. 1993. Distributed Morphology and the Pieces of Inflection. In The View from Building 20: Essays in Linguistics in Honor of Sylvain Bromberger, ed. Kenneth Hale and Samuel Keyser, 111–176. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, James W. 1969. Spanish Phonology. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 1985. Spanish Diphthongisation and Stress: A Paradox Resolved. Phonology 2: 31–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hart, Betty, and Todd R. Risley. 1995. Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hetzron, Robert. 1975. Where the Grammar Fails. Language 51: 859–872.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, Archibald A. 1976. [Obituary:] Albert Henry Marckwardt. Language 52: 667–681.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hudson, Richard. 2000. *I amn’t. Language 76: 297–323.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiparsky, Paul. 1973. “Elsewhere” in Phonology. In A Festschrift for Morris Halle, ed. Stephen R. Anderson and Paul Kiparsky, 93–106. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kottum, Steiner S. 1981. The Genitive Singular form of Masculine Nouns in Polish. Scando-Slavica 27: 179–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Labov, William. 1972. Sociolinguistic Patterns. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Labov, William, Sharon Ash, and Charles Boberg. 2006. The Atlas of North American English: Phonetics, Phonology, and Sound Change. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, Marielle. 2011. Spanish 3rd Conjugation Verb Stem Alternations. Ms., University of Pennsylvania.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacWhinney, Brian. 2000. The CHILDES Project: Tools for Analyzing Talk, 3rd ed. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maiden, Martin, and Paul O’Neill. 2010. On Morphomic Defectiveness: Evidence from the Romance Languages of the Iberian Peninsula. In Defective Paradigms: Missing Forms and What They Tell Us, ed. Matthew Baerman, Greville G. Corbett, and Dunstan Brown, 103–124. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcus, Gary. 1995. Children’s Overregularization of English Plurals: A Quantitative Analysis. Journal of Child Language 22: 447–459.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marcus, Gary, Steven Pinker, Michael T. Ullman, Michelle Hollander, John Rosen, and Fei Xu. 1992. Overregularization in Language Acquisition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcus, Gary F., Ursula Brinkmann, Harald Clahsen, Richard Wiese, and Steven Pinker. 1995. German Inflection: The Exception that Proves the Rule. Cognitive Psychology 29: 189–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mateo, Francis, and Antonio Rojo Sastre. 1995. El arte de conjugar en español. Paris: Hatier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maunsch, Hanna. 2003. Current Alternations in Inflection of Polish Masculine Inanimate Nouns in the Singular: A Pilot Study. Investigationes Linguisticae 9: 4–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayol, Laia. 2007. Acquisition of Irregular Patterns in Spanish Verbal Morphology. In Proceedings of the Twelfth ESSLLI Student Session, ed. Ville Nurmi and Dmitry Sustretov, 1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merkuur, Anne, Jan Don, Eric Hoekstra, and Arjen P. Versloot. 2019. Competition in Frisian Past Participles. In Competition in Inflection and Word-Formation, ed. Franz Rainer, Francesco Gardani, Wolfgang U. Dressler, and Hans Christian Luschützky, 195–222. Cham: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mills, Anne. 1986. The Acquisition of Gender: A Study of English and German. Berlin: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Moskvin, Vadim. 2015. Nedostatochnye Glagoly: k utochneniju pon’atija. Russian Linguistics 39: 209–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, William E., and Anderson, Richard C. 1984. How Many Words Are There in Printed School English? Reading Research Quarterly 19: 304–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ———. 1866. Formenlehre der lateinischen Sprache. Stuttgart: H. Lindemann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newport, Elissa L. 1990. Maturational Constraints on Language. Cognitive Science 14: 11–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nida, Eugene A. 1949. Morphology: The Descriptive Analysis of Words, 2nd ed. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Orgun, Orhan C., and Ronald Sprouse. 1999. From MPARSE to CONTROL: Deriving Ungrammaticality. Phonology 16: 191–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pertsova, Katya. 2005. How Lexical Conservatism Can Lead to Paradigm Gaps. In UCLA Working Papers in Linguistics 11: Papers in Phonology 6, 13–30. University of California, Los Angeles.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2016. Transderivational Relations and Paradigm Gaps in Russian Verbs. Glossa 1: 13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, Steven. 1999. Words and Rules: The Ingredients of Language. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prince, Alan, and Paul Smolensky. 1993. Optimality Theory: Constraint Interaction in Generative Grammar. Technical Report TR-2, Rutgers University Center For Cognitive Science and Technical Report CU-CS-533-91, University of Colorado, Boulder Department of Computer Science. Published by MIT Press, Cambridge, 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pullum, Geoffrey K., and Deidre Wilson. 1977. Autonomous Syntax and the Analysis of Auxiliaries. Language 53: 741–788.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raffelsiefen, Renate. 1996. Gaps in Word Formation. In Interfaces in Phonology, ed. Ursula Kleinhenz, 194–209. Berlin: Akademie-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 1999. Phonological Constraints on English Word Formation. In Yearbook of Morphology 1998, ed. Geert Booij and Jaap van Marle, 225–287. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Real Academia Española. 1992. Diccionario de la lengua española, 21st ed. Madrid: Real Academia Española.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rebrus, Péter, and Miklós Törkenczy. 2009. Covert and Overt Defectiveness in Paradigms. In Modeling Ungrammaticality in Optimality Theory, ed. Sylvia Blaho and Curt Rice, 195–234. London: Equinox.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rice, Curt. 2005. Optimal Gaps in Optimal Paradigms. Catalan Journal of Linguistics 4: 155–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rice, Curt, and Sylvia Blaho, ed. 2009. Modeling Ungrammaticality in Optimality Theory. London: Equinox.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, Edward A. 2014. A Comprehensive Etymological Dictionary of the Spanish Language with Families of Words Based on Indo-European Roots, vol. 2. Bloomington: Xlibris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schuler, Kathryn D., Charles Yang, and Elissa L. Newport. 2016. Testing the Tolerance Principle: Children form Productive Rules When It Is More Computationally Efficient to Do So. In Proceedings of the 38th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, ed. Anna Papafragou, Daniel Grodner, Daniel Mirman, and John C. Trueswell, 2321–2326. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sebastián, Núria, Fernando Cuetos, Antònia Martí, and Manuel Carreiras. 2001. LEXESP: Léxico informatizado del español. Barcelona: Edicions de la Universitat de Barcelona.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sims, Andrea. 2006. Minding the Gap: Inflectional Defectiveness in a Paradigmatic Theory. Doctoral dissertation, Ohio State University.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2007. Why Defective Paradigms Are, and Aren’t, the Result of Competing Morphological Patterns. In Proceedings of the 43rd Annual Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society, 267–281. University of Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slioussar, Natalia, and Maria Kholodilova. 2013. Paradigm Leveling in Non-standard Russian. In Formal Approaches to Slavic Linguistics: The Second MIT Meeting 2011, ed. Alexander Podobryaev, 243–258. Ann Arbor: Michigan Slavic Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smoczyńska, Magdalena. 1985. The Acquisition of Polish. In The Crosslinguistic Study of Language Acquisition. Volume 1: The Data, ed. Daniel I. Slobin, 595–686. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smolensky, Paul. 1996. The Initial State and ‘Richness of the Base’ in Optimality Theory. Technical Report JHU-CogSci-96-4, Department of Cognitive Science, Johns Hopkins University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sonnenstuhl, Ingrid, and Axel Huth. 2002. Processing and Representation of German -n Plurals: A Dual Mechanism Approach. Brain and Language 81: 276–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, Andrew. 1991. Morphological Theory: An Introduction to Word Structure in Generative Grammar. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steriade, Donca. 1997. Lexical Conservatism. In Linguistics in the Morning Calm: Selected Papers from SICOL 1997, 157–179. Seoul: Hanshin.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2008. A Pseudo-Cyclic Effect in Romanian Morphophonology. In Inflectional Identity, ed. Asaf Bachrach and Andrew Nevins, 313–360. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Szagun, Gisela. 2001. Learning Different Regularities: The Acquisition of Noun Plurals by German-Speaking Children. First Language 21: 109–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2004. Learning by Ear: On the Acquisition of Case and Gender Marking by German-Speaking Children with Normal Hearing and with Cochlear Implants. Journal of Child Language 31: 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Versloot, Arjen P., and Eric Hoekstra. 2019. Blocking as a Function of the Nature of Linguistic Representations: Where Psycholinguistics and Morphology Meet. In Competition in Inflection and Word-Formation, ed. Franz Rainer, Francesco Gardani, Wolfgang U. Dressler, and Hans Christian Luschützky, 145–166. Cham: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weist, Richard M., and Katarzyna Witkowska-Stadnik. 1986. Basic Relations in Child Language and the Word Order Myth. International Journal of Psychology 21: 363–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weist, Richard M., Hanna Wysocka, Katarzyna Witkowska-Stadnik, Ewa Buczowska, and Emilia Konieczna. 1984. The Defective Tense Hypothesis: On the Emergence of Tense and Aspect in Child Polish. Journal of Child Language 11: 347–374.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiese, Richard. 2000. The Phonology of German. Oxford: Clarendon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, Matthew, and John McCarthy. 2009. Less Than Zero: Correspondence and the Null Output. In Modeling Ungrammaticality in Optimality Theory, ed. Sylvia Blaho and Curt Rice, 17–66. London: Equinox.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wunderlich, Dieter. 1999. German Noun Plural Reconsidered. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22: 1044–1045.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, Fei, and Steven Pinker. 1995. Weird Past Tense Forms. Journal of Child Language 22: 531–556.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yang, Charles. 2002. Knowledge and Learning in Natural Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2005. On Productivity. Linguistic Variation Yearbook 5: 333–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2010. Three Factors in Language Variation. Lingua 120: 1160–1177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2016. The Price of Linguistic Productivity. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2017. How to Wake Irregular (and Speechless). In On Looking into Words (and Beyond): Structures, Relations, Analyses, ed. Claire Bowern, Laurence Horn, and Raffaela Zanuttini, 211–232. Berlin: Language Science Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yip, Kenneth, and Gerald J. Sussman. 1997. Sparse Representations for Fast, One-Shot Learning. In Proceedings of the Fourteenth National Conference on Artificial Intelligence, 521–527, AAAI Press/The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kyle Gorman .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2019 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Gorman, K., Yang, C. (2019). When Nobody Wins. In: Rainer, F., Gardani, F., Dressler, W., Luschützky, H. (eds) Competition in Inflection and Word-Formation . Studies in Morphology, vol 5. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-02550-2_7

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-02550-2_7

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-030-02549-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-030-02550-2

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics