Skip to main content

The Neurophysiology of Early Motor Resonance

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Mirror Neuron Systems

Part of the book series: Contemporary Neuroscience ((CNEURO))

  • 2528 Accesses

Abstract

The discovery of mirror neurons in the monkey brain and the subsequent description of a mirror neuron system (MNS) in humans have triggered unprecedented interest in the neural basis of social cognition. Although the mechanism matching action observation and execution is now well documented in human adults, much remains to be learned about its normal development. In light of accumulating evidence suggesting a link between impaired motor resonance and developmental disorders such as autism spectrum disorder, it is of paramount importance to better understand how the MNS develops and under what specific conditions it appears. Until recently, speculation about the existence of a functioning MNS in the infant brain relied primarily on evidence of imitative behavior shortly after birth. Here, we describe studies that have tackled the issue of detailing MNS properties in the immature brain. Through a variety of neuroimaging techniques, a preliminary description of neurophysiological mechanisms underlying early imitation and action understanding is starting to emerge. We will see, however, that although important first steps have been taken, much remains to be learned about early mirroring processes and their relationship to social behavior.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Abravanel, E., & Sigafoos, A. D. (1984). Exploring the presence of imitation during early infancy. Child Development, 55, 381–392.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Anisfeld, M. (1996). Only tongue protrusion modelling is matched by neonates. Developmental Review, 16, 149–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anisfeld, M., Turkewitz, G., Rose, S., Rosenberg, F., Sheiber, F., & Couturier-Fagan, D. (2001). No compelling evidence that newborns imitate oral gestures. Infancy, 2, 111–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arbib, M. (2005). From monkey-like action recognition to human language: An evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics. Behavioral & Brain Science, 28, 105–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bekkering, H., Wohlschlager, A., & Gattis, M. (2000). Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A. 2000 Feb, 53(1), 153–64.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Blakemore, S. J.,& Choudhury, S. (2006). Development of the adolescent brain: Implications for executive function and social cognition. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 296–312.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D. E., Grezes, J., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2005). Action observation and acquired motor skills: An fmri study with expert dancers. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1243–1249.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, X., Striano, T., & Rakoczy, H. (2004). Auditory-oral matching behavior in newborns. Developmental Science, 7, 42–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Choudhury, S., Charman, T., Bird, V., & Blakemore, S. J. (2007). Development of action representation during adolescence. Neuropsychologia, 45, 255–262.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cross, E. S., Hamilton, A. F., & Grafton, S. T. (2006). Building a motor simulation de novo: Observation of dance by dancers. Neuroimage, 31, 1257–1267.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dapretto, M., Davies, M. S., Pfeifer, J. H., Scott, A. A., Sigman, M, Bookheimer, S. Y., et al. (2006). Understanding emotions in others: Mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 28–30.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Falck-Ytter, T., Gredeback, G., & von Hofsten, C. (2006). Infants predict other people’s action goals. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 878–879.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fecteau, S., Carmant, L., Tremblay, C., Robert, M., Bouthillier, A., & Theoret, H. (2004). A motor resonance mechanism in children? Evidence from subdural electrodes in a 36-month-old child. Neuroreport, 15, 2625–2627.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrari, P. F., Rozzi, S., & Fogassi, L. (2005). Mirror neurons responding to observation of actions made with tools in monkey ventral premotor cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 212–226.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Field, T. M., Woodson, R., Greenberg, R., & Cohen, D. (1982). Discrimination and imitation of facial expression by neonates. Science, 218, 179–181.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Field, T. M., Woodson, R., Cohen, D., Greenberg, R., Garcia, R., & Collins, E. (1983). Discrimination and imitation of facial expressions by term and preterm neonates. Infant Behavior and Development, 6, 485–489.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Field, T. M., Goldstein, S., Vaga-Lahr, N., & Porter, K. (1986). Changes in imitative behavior during early infancy. Infant Behavior and Development, 9, 415–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flanagan J. R., & Johansson R. S. (2003). Action plans used in action observation. Nature, 424, 769–771.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fontaine, R. (1984). Imitative skills between birth and six months. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 323–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119, 593–609.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., Keysers, C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2004). A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Science, 8, 396–403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, L., & Watson, J. (1981). Neonatal imitation: Fact or artifact. Developmental Psychology, 177, 660–665.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heimann, M., & Schaller, J. (1985). Imitative reactions among 14-sup-21 day old infants. Infant Mental Health Journal, 6, 31–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heimann, M., Nelson, K. E., & Schaller, J. (1989). Neonatal imitation of tongue protrusion and mouth opening: Methodological aspects and evidence of early individual differences. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 30, 90–101.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Heimann, M. (2002). Notes on individual differences and the assumed elusiveness of neonatal imitation. In: A. N. Meltzoff & W. Prinz, (Eds.), The imitative mind: Development evolution, and brain bases (pp. 74–84). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Heiser, M., Iacoboni, M., Maeda, F., Marcus, J., & Mazziotta, J. C. (2003). The essential role of Broca’s area in imitation. European Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 1123–1128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iacoboni, M., Woods, R. P., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Mazziotta, J. C., & Rizzolatti G. (1999). Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science, 286, 2526–2528.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobson, S. W. (1979). Matching behavior in the young infant. Child Development, 50, 425–430.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, S. S. (1996). Imitation or exploration? Young infants’ matching of adults’ oral gestures. Child Development, 67, 1952–1969.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, S. S. (2006). Exploration or imitation? The effect of music on 4-week-old infants’ tongue protrusions. Infant Behavior and Development, 29, 126–130.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kohler, E., Keysers, C., Umilta, M. A., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Hearing sounds, understanding actions: Action representation in mirror neurons. Science, 297, 846–848.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kugiumutzakis, G. (1999). Genesis and development of early infant mimesis to facial and vocal models. In: J. Nadel & G. Butterworth (Eds.), Imitation in infancy (pp. 36–59). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, P. K., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1982). The bimodal perception of speech in infancy. Science, 218, 1138–1141.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, P. K., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1996). Infant vocalizations in response to speech: Vocal imitation and developmental change. Journal of Acoustical Society of America, 100, 2425–2438.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, P. K., Williams, K. A., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1991). Cross-modal speech perception in adults and infants using nonspeech auditory stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception Perform, 17, 829–840.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Legerstee, M. (1991). The role of person and object in eliciting early imitation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 51, 423–433.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lepage, J., & Théoret, H. (2006). EEG evidence for the presence of an observation-execution matching system in children. European Journal of Neurology, 23, 2505–2510.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maratos, O. (1982). Trends in the development of imitation in early infancy. In: T. G. Bever (Ed.), Regressions in mental development: Basic phenomena and theories (pp. 81–101). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Decety, J. (2003). What imitation tells us about social cognition: A rapprochement between developmental psychology and cognitive neuroscience. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 358, 491–500.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 74–78.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1983). Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures. Child Development, 54, 702–709.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1989). Imitation in newborn infants: Exploring the range of gestures imitated and the underlying mechanisms. Developmental Psychology, 25, 954–962.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1992). Early imitation within a functional framework: The importance of person identity, movement, and development. Infant Behavior and Development, 15, 479–505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1997). Explaining facial imitation: A theoretical model. Early Developmental and Parenting, 6, 179–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muthukumaraswamy, S. D., Johnson, B. W., & McNair, N. A. (2004). Mu rhythm modulation during observation of an object-directed grasp. Brain Research Cognitive Brain Research, 19, 195–201.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, E., Compagne, H., Orvos, H., Pal, A., Molnar, P., Janszky, I., et al. (2005). Index finger movement imitation by human neonates: Motivation, learning, and left-hand preference. Pediatric Research, 58(4), 749–753.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oberman, L. M. H., Edward, M., McCleery, J. P., Altchuler, E. L., Ramachadran, V. S., & Pineda, J. A. (2005). Eeg evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. Cognitive Brain Research, 24, 190–198.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pechaux, M. G., Lepecq, J. C., & Salzarulo, P. (1988). Oral activity and exploration in 1–2 month-old infants. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6, 245–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pfeifer, H., Iacoboni, M., Mazziotta, C., & Dapretto, M. (2005). Mirror neuron system activity in children and its relation to empathy and interpersonal competence. In: Abstract Viewer/Itineraray Planner. Soc Neurosci Abstr, 660.24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International Universities Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Pineda, J. A. (2005). The functional significance of mu rhythms: Translating “seeing” and “hearing” into “doing”. Brain Research Review, 50, 57–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prechtl, H. F. (1986). Prenatal motor development. In M. G. Wade & H. T. Whiting (Eds.), Motor development in children: Aspects of coordination and control (pp. 53–64). Dordecht: Martinus Nijhoff.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reissland, N. (1988). Neonatal imitation in the first hour of life: Observations in rural Nepal. Developmental Psychology, 24, 464–469.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V., & Fogassi, L. (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Brain Research Cognitive Brain Research, 3, 131–141.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rochat, P. (1983). Oral touch in young infants: Response to variations of nipple characteristics in the first months of life. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 6, 123–133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saygin, A. P., Wilson, S. M., Dronkers, N. F., & Bates, E. (2004). Action comprehension in aphasia: Linguistic and non-linguistic deficits and their lesion correlates. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1788–1804.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shimada, S., & Hiraki, K. (2006). Infant’s brain responses to live and televised action. Neuroimage, 32, 930–939.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sommerville, J. A., Woodward, A. L., & Needham, A. (2005). Action experience alters 3-month-old infants’ perception of others’ actions. Cognition, 96, B1–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Théoret, H., Halligan, E., Kobayashi, M., Fregni, F., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2005). Impaired motor facilitation during action observation in individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Current Biology, 15, R84–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van der Meer, A. L., van der Weel, F. R., & Lee, D. N. (1995). The functional significance of arm movements in neonates. Science, 267, 693–695.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van der Meer, A. L. (1997). Keeping the arm in the limelight: Advanced visual control of arm movements in neonates. European Jounral of Pediatric Neurology, 1, 103–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wohlschlager, A., & Bekkering, H. (2002). Is human imitation based on a mirror-neurone system? Some behavioural evidence. Experimental Brain Research, 143, 335–341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We gratefully acknowledge financial support from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research, the National Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada, and the Fonds de Recherche en Santé du Québec.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Hugo Théoret .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2008 Humana Press, a part of Springer Science+Business Media, LLC

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Champoux, F., Lepage, JF., Désy, MC., Lortie, M., Théoret, H. (2008). The Neurophysiology of Early Motor Resonance. In: Pineda, J.A. (eds) Mirror Neuron Systems. Contemporary Neuroscience. Humana Press. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-59745-479-7_3

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics