Skip to main content

Embodied Perspective on Emotion-Cognition Interactions

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Mirror Neuron Systems

Part of the book series: Contemporary Neuroscience ((CNEURO))

Abstract

Emotions permeate social and non-social cognition. However, our understanding of the underlying mechanisms remains elusive. One reason is because traditional amodal or symbolic accounts of cognition view emotional information as equivalent to any other information. However, recent theories of embodied cognition suggest new ways to understand the processing of emotionally significant information. They suggest that both perceiving and thinking about such information involve perceptual, somatovisceral, and motoric reexperiences (embodiment) of the relevant emotion in the self. Consistent with this view, many studies show that processing of emotion recruits embodiments as reflected in psychological and psychological measures. Further, embodiment of emotion, even when induced by simple manipulations, such as facial expression, posture, or movement, can causally influence the processing of emotional information, including perception, learning, understanding, and use in language, judgment, and behavior. We review relevant studies and discuss potential neural mechanisms underlying embodiment and simulation. We especially highlight the importance of social context and flexible use of embodiment in emotional processing and discuss its importance for typical and atypical social functioning.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Adolphs, R., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., Cooper, G., & Damasio, A. (2000). A role for somatosensory cortices in the visual recognition of emotion as revealed by three-dimensional lesion mapping. Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 2683–2690.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol system. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577–660.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W. (2003). Abstraction in perceptual symbol systems. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Biological Sciences, 358, 1177–1187.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Batson, C. D. (1991). The altruism question: Toward a social-psychological answer. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., Fink, G. R., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., et al. (2001). Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in somatotopic manner: An fMRI study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 400–404.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., Martzke, J., & Tassinary, L. G. (1988). Specific forms of facial EMG response index emotions during an interview: From Darwin to the continuous flow model of affect-laden information processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 592–604.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J. T., Priester, J. R., & Berntson, G. G. (1993). Rudimentary determinants of attitudes: II. Arm flexion and extension have differential effects on attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 5–17.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Calder, A. J., Keane, J., Cole, J., Campbell, R., & Young, A. W. (2000). Facial expression recognition by people with Mobius syndrome. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17, 73–87.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cannon W. B. (1927). The James-Lange theory of emotions. American Journal of Psychology, 39, 115–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carr, L., Iacoboni, M., Dubeau, M. C., Mazziotta, J. C., and Lenzi G. L. (2003). Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: A relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA, 100, 5497–5502.

    Google Scholar 

  • Centerbar, D., & Clore, G. L. (2006). Do approach-avoidance actions create attitudes? Psychological Science, 17, 22–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893–910.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, S., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). Consequences of automatic evaluation: Immediate behavior predispositions to approach or avoid the stimulus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 215–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (1997). Being there: Putting brain body and world together again. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craig, A. D. (2002). How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 655–66.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R. (1989). Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: A systems-level proposal for the neural substrates of recall and recognition. Cognition, 33, 25–62.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Grosset/Putnam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R., Grabowski, T. J., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Ponto, L. L. B., Parvizi, J., et al. (2000). Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feeling of self-generated emotions. Nature Neuroscience, 3,1049–1056.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dapretto, M., Davies, M. S., Pfeifer, J. H., Scott, A. A., Sigman, M., Bookheimer, S.Y., et al. (2005). Understanding emotions in others: mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nature neuroscience, 9(1), 28–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Decety, J., & Jackson, P. L. (2004). The functional architecture of human empathy. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 3, 71–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dimberg, U., Thunberg, M., & Elmehed, K. (2000). Unconscious facial reactions to emotional facial expressions. Psychological Science, 11, 86–89.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Eich, E., Kihlstrom, J., Bower, G., Forgas, J., & Niedenthal, P. (2000). Cognition and emotion. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellsworth, P. C., & Scherer, K. R. (2003). Appraisal processes in emotion. In R. J. Davidson, H. Goldsmith, & K. R. Scherer (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences. New York and Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. (1975). The language of thought. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J., & Pylyshyn, Z. (1988). Connectionism and cognitive architecture: A critical analysis. Cognition, 28, 3–71.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Forgas, J. P. (2002). Feeling and doing: The role of affect in social cognition and behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 9, 205–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. (2003). The roots of empathy: The shared manifold hypothesis and the neural basis of intersubjectivity. Psychopathology, 36, 71–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., Keysers, C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2004). A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 396–402.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M., & Robinson, D. A. (2000). Symbol grounding and meaning: A comparison of high-dimensional and embodied theories of meaning. Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 379–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, A. I., & Sripada, C. S. (2005). Simulationist models of face-based emotion recognition, Cognition, 94, 193–213.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grush, R. 2004 The emulation theory of representation: motor control, imagery, and perception. Behavioral Brain Sciences, 27, 377–396.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hadjikhani, N., Joseph, R. M., Snyder, J., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2006). Anatomical differences in the mirror neuron system and social cognition network in autism. Cerebral Cortex, 9, 1276–1282.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, A., Brindley, R. M., & Frith U. (2007). Imitation and Action Understanding in Autistic Spectrum Disorders: how valid is the hypothesis of a deficit in the mirror neuron system? Neuropsychologia, 45, 1859–1868.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Havas, D. A., Glenberg, A. M., & Rinck, M. (2007). Emotion simulation during language comprehension. Psychonomic Bullentin & Review, 14, 436–441.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hess, U., Philippot, P., & Blairy, S., (1999). Facial mimicry: Facts and fiction. In P. Philippot, R. Feldman, & E. Coats (Eds.), The social context of nonverbal behavior (pp. 213–241). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heyes, C. M. (2001). Causes and consequences of imitation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 253–261.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heyes, C. M., Bird, G., Johnson, H., & Haggard, P. (2005). Experience modulates automatic imitation. Cognitive Brain Research, 22, 233–240.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hutchison, W. D., Davis, K. D., Lozano, A. M., Tasker, R. R., & Dostrovsky, J. O. (1999). Pain related neurons in the human cingulate cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 403–405.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Iacoboni, M., Koski, L., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Woods, R. P., Dubeau, M. -C., et al. (2001). Re-afferent Copies of Imitated Actions in the Right Superior Temporal Cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 98, 13995–13999.

    Google Scholar 

  • James W. (1884). What is an emotion? Mind, 9, 188–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knutson, B., Wimmer, G. E., Kuhnen, C. M., & Winkielman, P. (2008). Nucleus accumbens activation mediates the influence of reward cues on financial risk-taking. NeuroReport, 19, 509–513.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., & Núñez, R. (2000). Where mathematics comes from: How the embodied mind erings mathematics into being. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larsen, J. T., Norris, C. J., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2003). Effects of positive affect and negative affect on electromyographic activity over zygomaticus major and corrugator supercilii. Psychophysiology, 40, 776–785.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lipps, T. (1907). Das Wissen von fremden Ichen. In T. Lipps (Ed.), Psychologische Untersuchungen (Band 1) (pp. 694–722). Leipzig: Engelmann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markman, A. B., & Brendl, C.M. (2005), Constraining theories of embodied cognition, Psychological Science, 16, 6–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McIntosh, D. N. (2006). Spontaneous facial mimicry, liking and emotional contagion. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 37, 31–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • McIntosh, D. N., Reichmann-Decker, A., Winkielman, P., & Wilbarger, J. (2006). When mirroring fails: Deficits in spontaneous, but not controlled mimicry of emotional facial expressions in autism. Developmental Science, 9, 295–302.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Niedenthal, P.M., Winkielman, P., Mondillon, L., & Vermeulen, N. (under review). Embodiment of emotion concepts: Evidence from EMG measures.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, R., & Strack, F. (2000). “Mood contagion”: The automatic transfer of mood between persons. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 211–223.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A. (1980). Physical symbol systems. Cognitive Science, 4, 135–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Niedenthal, P. M. (2007). Embodying emotion. Science, 316, 1002–1005.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Niedenthal, P. M., Barsalou, L. W., Winkielman, P., Krauth-Gruber, S., & Ric, F. (2005). Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9, 184–211.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Niedenthal, P. M., Barsalou, L. W., Ric, F., & Krauth-Gruber, S. (2005). Embodiment in the acquisition and use of emotion knowledge. In L. Feldman Barrett, P. M. Niedenthal, & P. Winkielman (Eds.), Emotion: Conscious and unconscious (pp. 21–50). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Niedenthal, P. M., Brauer, M., Halberstadt, J. B., & Innes-Ker, Å. (2001). When did her smile drop? Facial mimicry and the influences of emotional state on the detection of change in emotional expression. Cognition and Emotion, 15, 853–864.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Niedenthal, P. M., Rohman, A., & Dalle, N. (2002). What is primed by emotion words and emotion concepts? In J. Musch & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion. Nahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nishitani, N., Avikainen, S., & Hari, R. (2004). Abnormal imitation-related cortical activation sequences in Asperger's syndrome. Annuls of Neurology, 55, 558–562.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oberman, L. M., Hubbard, E. M., McCleery, J. P., Ramachandran, V. S., & Pineda, J. A. (2005). EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism. Cognitive Brain Research, 24, 190–198.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oberman, L. M., & Ramachandran, V. S. (2007). The simulating social mind: The role of simulation in the social and communicative deficits of autism spectrum disorders. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 310–327.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oberman, L. M., Winkielman, P., & Ramachandran, V.S. (2007). Face to Face: Blocking expression-specific muscles can selectively impair recognition of emotional faces. Social Neuroscience, 12, 167–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pecher, D., Zeelenberg, R., & Barsalou, L. W. (2003). Verifying different-modality properties for concepts produces switching costs. Psychological Science, 14, 119–124.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Phelps, E. A., O’Connor, K. J., Gateby, J. J., Grillon, C., Gore, J. C., & Davis, M. (2001). Activation of the amygdala by cognitive representations of fear. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 437–441.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, J. J. (2002). Furnishing the mind: Concepts and their perceptual basis. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reed, C. L., & McGoldrick, J. E. (2007). Action during body perception: Processing time affects self–other correspondences. Social Neuroscience, 2, 1747.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. R., & Semin, G. R. (2007). Situated social cognition. Current Directions In Psychological Science, 16, 132–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singer, T., Seymour, B., O'Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R. J., Frith, C. D. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science, 303, 1157–1162.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Singer, T., Seymour, B., O'Doherty, J., Klaas, E.S., Dolan, J.D., & Frith, C. (2006). Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others. Nature, 439, 466–469.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, K. O., & Barsalou, L. W. (2004). Perceptual simulation in property verification. Memory & Cognition, 32, 244–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Solarz, A. K. (1960). Latency of instrumental responses as a function of compatibility with the meaning of eliciting verbal signs. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 239–245.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, C., Nicholls, M. E., & Driver, J. (2001). The cost of expecting events in the wrong sensory modality. Perception & Psychophysics, 63, 330–336.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Strack, F., Martin, L. L., & Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: A nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 768–777.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Strack, F., Schwarz, N., & Gschneidinger, E. (1985). Happiness and reminiscing: The role of time perspective, mood, and mode of thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1460–1469.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tassinary, L. G., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2000). The skeletomuscular system: Surface electromyography. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G. G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology, (2nd ed., pp. 163–199). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Theoret, H., Halligan, E., Kobayashi, M., Fregni, F., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2005). Impaired motor facilitation during action observation in individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Current Biology, 15, R84–R85.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 830–846.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • van Baaren, R. B., Maddux, W. W., Chartrand, T. L., de Bouter, C., & van Knippenberg, A. (2003). It takes two to mimic: Behavioral consequences of self-construals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 1093–1102.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vermeulen, N., Niedenthal, P.M., & Luminet, O. (2007). Switching between sensory and affective systems incurs processing costs. Cognitive Science, 31, 183–192.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Villalobos, M. E., Mizuno, A., Dahl, B. C., Kemmotsu, N., & Muller, R. A. (2005). Reduced functional connectivity between V1 and inferior frontal cortex associated with visuomotor performance in autism. Neuroimage, 25, 916–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wallbott, H. G. (1991). Recognition of emotion from facial expression via imitation? some indirect evidence for an old theory. British Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 207–219.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G. L., & Petty, R. E. (1980). The effects of overt head movements on persuasion: Compatibility and incompatibility of responses. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 1, 219–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wentura, D., & Rothermund, K., & Bak, P. (2000). Automatic vigilance: The attention-grabbing power of approach- and avoidance-related social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 1024–1037.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. H. G., Whiten, A., & Singh, T. (2004). A systematic review of action imitation in autistic spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 34, 285–299.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 625–636.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wicker, B., Keysers, C., Plailly, J., Royet, J. P., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2003). Both of us disgusted in My insula: the common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron, 40, 655–664.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Winkielman, P., Berridge, K. C., & Wilbarger, J. L. (2005). Unconscious affective reactions to masked happy versus angry faces influence consumption behavior and judgments of value. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 1, 121–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winkielman, P., Knutson, B., Paulus, M., Trujillo, J. L. (2007). Affective influence on judgments and decisions: Moving towards core mechanisms. Review of General Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winkielman, P., Schwarz, N., Fazendeiro, T., & Reber, R. (2003). The hedonic marking of processing fluency: Implications for evaluative judgment. In J. Musch & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion. (pp. 189–217). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. B., Adelmann, P. K., Murphy, S. T., & Niedenthal, P. M. (1987). Convergence in the physical appearance of spouses. Motivation and Emotion, 11, 335–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. B., & Markus, H. (1984). Affect and cognition: The hard interface. In C. Izard, J. Kagan, & R. B. Zajonc (Eds.), Emotions, cognition and behavior (pp. 73–102). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zwaan, R. A. (2004). The immersed experiencer: Toward an embodied theory of language comprehension. In B. H. Ross (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 44, 35–62.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We appreciate discussions with Adam Aron, Larry Barsalou, Lisa Barrett, Thierry Chaminade, Vic Ferreira, Chris Frith, Celia Heyes, Dave Huber, Hal Pashler, Vilayanur Ramachandran, Cathy Reed, Norbert Schwarz, Gun Semin, Eliot Smith, and Robert Zajonc. This work is supported by NSF grant BCS-0350687 to Piotr Winkielman and Paula Niedenthal.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Piotr Winkielman .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2008 Humana Press, a part of Springer Science+Business Media, LLC

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Winkielman, P., Niedenthal, P.M., Oberman, L.M. (2008). Embodied Perspective on Emotion-Cognition Interactions. In: Pineda, J.A. (eds) Mirror Neuron Systems. Contemporary Neuroscience. Humana Press. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-59745-479-7_11

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics