Skip to main content

The Assessment of Skill Learning in African Children

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Neuropsychology of Children in Africa

Part of the book series: Specialty Topics in Pediatric Neuropsychology ((STPN))

Abstract

Procedural memory refers to the long-term memory system subserving the acquisition and retention of skills and habits and relying on a system of brain structures including corticostriatal circuitry. Not only is this system important in gaining new motor skills, but also to the acquisition of skills and habits involved in implicit knowledge of language rules and academic achievements. Although procedural memory plays a fundamental role in development, tasks specifically addressing this system are not commonly included in cognitive batteries developed to study neurodevelopmental factors in children from resource-poor settings. This chapter reviews age-appropriate and culturally sensitive approaches to the measurement of skill learning that involve both paper-and-pencil testing and computer-based assessment and demonstrates their utility and the complimentary information that they bring to neurodevelopmental studies of African children.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Adi-Japha, E., Fox, O., & Karni, A. (2011a). Atypical acquisition and atypical expression of memory consolidation gains in a motor skill in young female adults with ADHD. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32(3), 1011–1020.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Adi-Japha, E., Landau, Y., Frenkel, L., Teicher, M., Gross-Tzur, V., & Shalev, R. S. (2007). ADHD and dysgraphia: Underlying mechanisms. Cortex, 43, 700–709.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Adi-Japha, E., Strulovich-Schwartz, O., & Julius, M. (2011b). Delayed motor skill acquisition in children with language impairment. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32, 2963–2971.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Adolph, K. (2005). Learning to learn in the development of action. In J. J. Rieser, J. J. Lockman, & C. A. Nelson (Eds.), Action as an organizer of learning and development (pp. 91–122). Mawah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Adolph, K. (2008). Learning to move. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 213–218.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Adolph, K., & Berger, S. A. (2006). Motor development. In W. Damon & R. Lerner (Series Eds.) & D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Cognition, perception, and language (pp. 161–213). New York, NY: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed. text revision ed). Washington, DC: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnes, K. A., Howard, J. H., Jr., Howard, D. V., Gilotty, L., Kenworthy, L., Gaillard, W. D., et al. (2008). Intact implicit learning of spatial context and temporal sequences in childhood autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychology, 22, 563–570.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barnes, K. A., Howard, J. H., Howard, D. V., Kenealy, L., & Vaidya, C. J. (2010). Two forms of implicit learning in childhood ADHD. Developmental Neuropsychology, 35, 494–505.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1979). The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bebko, J. M., & Luhaorg, H. (1998). The development of strategy use and metacognitive processing in mental retardation: Some sources of difficulty. In J. A. Burack, R. M. Hodapp, & E. Zigler (Eds.), Handbook of mental retardation and development (pp. 382–407). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berger, K. S. (1988). The developing person through the life span (2nd ed.). New York: Worth Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Birdsong, D. (2006). Age and second language acquisition and processing: A selective overview. Language Learning, 56(S1), 9–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M. (2002). Motor immaturity and specific speech and language impairment: Evidence for a common genetic basis. American Journal of Medical Genetics: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 114, 56–63.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M., & Edmundson, A. (1987). Specific language impairment as a maturational lag: Evidence from longitudinal data on language and motor development. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 29, 442–459.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, L. (1993). The transition from infancy to language. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boivin, M. J., & Giordani, B. (2009). Neuropsychological assessment of African children: Evidence for a universal brain/behavior omnibus within a coconstructivist paradigm. In J. Y. Chiao (Ed.), Progress in brain research (Cultural neuroscience: Cultural influences on brain function, Vol. 178, pp. 113–135). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Born, J., & Wagner, U. (2004). Awareness in memory: Being explicit about the role of sleep. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 242–244.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bray, N. W. (1979). Strategy production in the retarded. In N. R. Ellis (Ed.), Handbook of mental deficiency, psychological theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 699–726). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bremner, A. J., Mareschal, D., Destrebecqz, A., & Cleeremans, A. (2007). Cognitive control of sequence knowledge in 2-year-olds. Psychological Science, 18, 261–266.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A. L. (1974). The role of strategic behavior in retardate memory. In N. R. Ellis (Ed.), International review of research in mental retardation (Vol. 7, pp. 55–111). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. M., & Robertson, E. M. (2007). Off-line processing: Reciprocal interactions between declarative and procedural memories. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 10468–10475.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Campos, J. J., Anderson, D. I., Barbu-Roth, M. A., Hubbard, E. M., Hertenstein, M. J., & Witherington, D. (2000). Travel broadens the mind. Infancy, 1, 149–219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, N. J., & Squire, L. R. (1980). Preserved learning and retention of pattern analyzing skill in amnesia: Dissociation of knowing how and knowing that. Science, 210, 207–210.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Corriveau, K. H., & Goswami, U. (2009). Rhythmic motor entrainment in children with speech and language impairments: Tapping to the beat. Cortex, 45, 119–130.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, A. (2000). Close interrelation of motor development and cognitive development and of the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex. Child Development, 71, 44–56.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dorfberger, S., Adi-Japha, E., & Karni, A. (2007). Reduced susceptibility to interference in the consolidation of motor memory before adolescence. PLoS ONE, 2, e240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doyon, J., Bellec, P., Amsel, R., Penhune, V., Monchi, O., Carrier, J., et al. (2009). Contributions of the basal ganglia and functionally related brain structures to motor learning. Behavioral Brain Research, 199, 61–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doyon, J., & Benali, H. (2005). Reorganization and plasticity in the adult brain during learning of motor skills. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15, 161–167.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Drummey, A., & Newcombe, N. (1995). Remembering versus knowing the past: Children’s explicit and implicit memories for pictures. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 59, 549–565.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, G. J., Dowsett, C. J., Claessens, A., Magnuson, K., Huston, A. C., Klebanov, P., et al. (2007). School readiness and later achievement. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1428–1446.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Estil, L. B., Whiting, H. T. A., Sigmundsson, H., & Ingvaldsen, R. P. (2003). Why might language and motor impairments occur together? Infant Child Development, 12, 253–265.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferman, S., & Karni, A. (2010). No childhood advantage in the acquisition of skill in using an artificial language rule. PLoS One, 5(10), e13648. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0013648

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, S., Wilhelm, I., & Born, J. (2007). Developmental differences in sleep’s role for implicit off-line learning: comparing children with adults. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(2), 214–227.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gheysen, F., Van Waelvelde, H., & Fias, W. (2011). Impaired visuo-motor sequence learning in developmental coordination disorder. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32, 749–756.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grissmer, D., Grimm, K. J., Aiyer, S. M., Murrah, W. M., & Steele, J. S. (2010). Fine motor skills and early comprehension of the world: Two new school readiness indicators. Developmental Psychology, 46, 1008–1017.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hatfield, G. (2003). Routledge philosophy guidebook to descartes and the “meditations”. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauptmann, B., Reinhart, E., Brandt, S. A., & Karni, A. (2005). The predictive value of the leveling off of within session performance for procedural memory consolidation. Cognitive Brain Research, 24, 181–189.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hedenius, M., Persson, J., Tremblay, A., Adi-Japha, E., Veríssimo, J., Dye, C. D., et al. (2011). Grammar predicts procedural learning and consolidation deficits in children with specific ­language impairment. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32, 2362–2375.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, E. (2001). Non-specific nature of specific language impairment: A review of the literature with regard to concomitant motor impairments. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 36, 149–171.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C., & Graham, A. (2002). Measuring executive functions in childhood: Problems and solutions? Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 7, 131–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huyck, J. J., & Wright, B. A. (2011). Late maturation of auditory perceptual learning. Developmental Science, 14, 614–621.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iverson, J. M. (2010). Developing language in a developing body: The relationship between motor development and language development. Journal of Child Language, 37, 229–261.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kantak, S. S., Sullivan, K. J., Fisher, B. E., Knowlton, B. J., & Winstein, C. J. (2010). Neural substrate of motor memory consolidation depend on practice structure. Nature Neuroscience, 13, 923–925.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Karasik, L. B., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Adolph, E. (2011). Transition from crawling to walking and infants' actions with objects and people. Child Development, 82, 1199–2009.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Karatekin, C., Markiewicz, S. W., & Siegel, M. A. (2003). A preliminary study of motor problems in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Perceptual and Motor Skills, 97, 1267–1280.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Karni, A., Meyer, G., Jezzard, P., Adams, M. M., Turner, R., & Ungerleider, L. G. (1995). Functional MRI evidence for adult motor cortex plasticity during motor skill-learning. Nature, 377, 155–158.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (2004). Manual for the Kaufman assessment battery for children (2nd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: AGS Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keuroghlian, A. S., & Knudsen, E. I. (2007). Adaptive auditory plasticity in developing and adult animals. Progress in Neurobiology, 82, 109–121.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koopmans-van Beinum, F. J., & van der Stelt, J. M. (1986). Early stages in the development of speech movements. In B. Lindbom & R. Zetterstrom (Eds.), Precursors of early speech (pp. 37–50). New York: Stockton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Korman, M., Doyon, J., Doljansky, J., Carrier, J., Dagan, Y., & Karni, A. (2007). Daytime sleep condenses the time course of motor memory consolidation. Nature Neuroscience, 10, 1206–1213.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Korman, M., Raz, N., Flash, T., & Karni, A. (2003). Multiple shifts in the representation of motor sequence during the acquisition of skilled performance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100, 12492–12497.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kovacs, A. M., & Mehler, J. (2009). Flexible learning of multiple speech structures in bilingual infants. Science, 325, 611–612.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lenneberg, E. H. (1967). Biological foundations of language. New York, NY: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., & Loring, D. W. (2004). Neuropsychological assessment (4th ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mather, D. S. (2003). Dyslexia and dysgraphia: More than written language difficulties in common. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36, 307–317.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meulemans, T., Van der Linden, M., & Perruchet, P. (1998). Implicit sequence learning in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 69, 199–221.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, C., Kail, R., Leonard, L., & Tomblin, J. B. (2001). Speed of processing in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 416–433.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Missiuna, C., Moll, S., Law, M., King, S., & King, G. (2006). Mysteries and mazes: Parents’ experiences of children with developmental coordination disorder. Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy, 73, 7–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex ‘frontal lobe’ tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mostofsky, S. H., Rimrodt, S. L., Schafer, J. G. B., Boyce, A., Goldberg, M. C., Pekar, J. J., et al. (2006). Atypical motor and sensory cortex activation in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of simple sequential finger tapping. Biological Psychiatry, 59, 48–56.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, K., McKone, E., & Slee, J. (2003). Dissociations between implicit and explicit memory in children: The role of strategic processing and the knowledge base. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 84, 124–165.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nemeth, D., Janacsek, K., Balogh, V., Londe, Z., Mingesz, R., Fazekas, M., et al. (2010). Learning in autism: Implicitly superb. PLoS One, 5(7), e11731. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0011731

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nicolson, R. I., Daum, I., Schugens, M. M., Fawcett, A. J., & Schulz, A. (2002). Eyeblink conditioning indicates cerebellar abnormality in dyslexia. Experimental Brain Research, 143, 42–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (2000). Long-term learning in dyslexic children. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 12, 357–393.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (2007). Procedural learning difficulties: Reuniting the developmental disorders? Trends in Neurosciences, 30, 135–141.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (2011). Dyslexia, dysgraphia, procedural learning and the cerebellum. Cortex, 47, 117–127.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nicolson, R. I., Fawcett, A. J., Berry, E. L., Jenkins, I. H., Dean, P., & Brooks, D. J. (1999). Association of abnormal cerebellar activation with motor learning difficulties in dyslexic adults. Lancet, 353, 1662–1667.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oller, D. K., & Eilers, R. E. (1988). The role of audition in infant babbling. Child Development, 59, 441–66.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children (trans. M. Cook). New York, NY: International Universities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1966). La psychologie de l’enfant [The psychology of the child]. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pianta, R. C., & McCoy, S. J. (1997). The first day of school: The predictive validity of early school screening. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 18, 1–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Polatajko, H. J., & Cantin, N. (2005). Developmental coordination disorder (dyspraxia): An overview of the state of the art. Seminars in Pediatric Neurology, 12(4), 250–258.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Poldrack, R. A., & Packard, M. G. (2003). Competition among multiple memory systems: Converging evidence from animal and human brain studies. Neuropsychologia, 41, 245–251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Powell, R., & Bishop, D. V. M. (1992). Clumsiness and perceptual problems in children with specific language impairment. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 34, 755–765.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reber, A. S. (1993). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reber, P. J., & Squire, L. R. (1998). Encapsulation of implicit and explicit memory in sequence learning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 248–263.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reis, J., Robertson, E. M., Krakauer, J. W., Rothwell, J., Marshall, L., Gerloff, C., et al. (2008). Consensus: Can transcranial direct current stimulation and transcranial magnetic stimulation enhance motor learning and memory formation? Brain Stimulation, 1, 363–369.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rey, A. (1958). L’examin clinique en psychologie. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riethmuller, A. M., Jones, R. A., & Okely, A. D. (2009). Efficacy of interventions to improve motor development in young children: A systematic review. Pediatrics, 124, E782–E792.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robertson, E. M. (2009). From creation to consolidation: A novel framework for memory processing. PLoS Biology, 7(1), e1000019. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1000019

    Google Scholar 

  • Roebers, C. M., & Kauer, M. (2008). Motor and cognitive control in a normative sample of 7-year-olds. Developmental Science, 12, 175–181.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruel, T. D., Boivin, M. J., Boal, H. E., Bangirana, P., Charlebois, E., Havlir, D. V., et al. (2012). Neurocognitive and motor deficits in HIV-infected Ugandan children with high CD4 cell counts. Clinical Infectious Diseases, 54, 1001–1009.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Savion-Lemieux, T., Bailey, J., & Penhune, V. (2009). Developmental contributions to motor sequence learning. Experimental Brain Research, 195, 293–306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seger, C. A. (2006). The basal ganglia in human learning. Neuroscientist, 12, 285–290.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smits Engelsman, B. C. M., & Van Galen, G. P. (1997). Dysgraphia in children: Lasting psychomotor deficiency or transient developmental delay? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 67, 164–184.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Son, S. H., & Meisels, S. J. (2006). The relationship of young children's motor skills to later reading and math achievement. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly: Journal of Developmental Psychology, 52, 755–778.

    Google Scholar 

  • Song, S., James, H., Howard, J., & Howard, D. V. (2007). Implicit probabilistic sequence learning is independent of explicit awareness. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27(46), 12475–12483.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Squire, L. R., Stark, C. E., & Clark, R. E. (2004). The medial temporal lobe. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 279–306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ste-Marie, D. M., Clark, S. E., Findlay, L. C., & Latimer, A. E. (2004). High levels of contextual interferences enhance handwriting skill acquisition. Journal of Motor Behaviour, 36(1), 115–126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thelen, E. (1979). Rhythmical stereotypies in normal human infants. Animal Behaviour, 27, 699–715.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, K. M., Hunt, R. H., Vizueta, N., Sommer, T., Durston, S., Yang, Y., et al. (2004). Evidence of developmental differences in implicit sequence learning: An fMRI study of children and adults. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1339–1351.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, K. M., & Nelson, C. A. (2001). Serial reaction time learning in preschool- and school-age children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 79, 364–387.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomblin, B., Mainela-Arnold, E., & Zhang, X. (2007). Procedural learning in adolescents with and without specific language impairment. Journal of Child Language Learning and Development, 3, 269–293.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomblin, B. J., Records, N., Buckwalter, P., Zhang, X., Smith, E., & O’Brien, M. (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 1245–1260.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ullman, M. T. (2004). Contributions of memory circuits to language: The declarative/procedural model. Cognition, 92(1–2), 231–270.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ullman, M. T., & Pierpont, E. I. (2005). Specific language impairment is not specific to language: The procedural deficit hypothesis. Cortex, 41, 399–433.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vicari, S. (2006). Motor development and neuropsychological patterns in persons with Down syndrome. Behavior Genetics, 36, 355–364.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vicari, S., Bellucci, S., & Carlesimo, G. A. (2000). Implicit and explicit memory: A functional dissociation in persons with Down syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 38, 240–251.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vicari, S., Finzi, A., Menghini, D., Marotta, L., Baldi, S., & Petrosini, L. (2005). Do children with developmental dyslexia have an implicit learning deficit? Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 76, 1392–1397.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vinter, A., & Detable, C. (2003). Implicit learning in children and adolescents with mental retardation. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 108, 94–107.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vinter, A., & Detable, C. (2008). Implicit and explicit motor learning in children with and without Down’s syndrome. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 26, 507–523.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vinter, A., & Perruchet, P. (2000). Implicit learning in children is not related to age: Evidence from drawing behavior. Child Development, 71, 1223–1240.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wassenberg, R., Feron, F., Kessels, A., Hendriksen, J., Kalff, A., Kroes, M., et al. (2005). Relation between cognitive and motor performance in 5- to 6-year-old children: results from a largescale cross-sectional study. Child Development, 76, 1092–1103.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watanabe, D., Savion-Lemieux, T., & Penhune, V. (2007). The effect of early musical training on adult motor performance: evidence for a sensitive period in motor learning. Experimental Brain Research, 176, 332–340.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilhelm, I., Diekelmann, S., & Born, J. (2008). Sleep in children improves memory performance on declarative but not procedural tasks. Learning & Memory, 15, 373–377.

    Google Scholar 

  • Windsor, J., Kohnert, K., Loxtercamp, A. L., & Kan, P. F. (2008). Performance on nonlinguistic visual tasks by children with language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29, 237–268.

    Google Scholar 

  • World Federation of Neurology. (1968). Report of research group on dyslexia and world illiteracy. Dallas, TX: Author.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Esther Adi-Japha Ph.D. .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2013 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Adi-Japha, E. (2013). The Assessment of Skill Learning in African Children. In: Boivin, M., Giordani, B. (eds) Neuropsychology of Children in Africa. Specialty Topics in Pediatric Neuropsychology. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-6834-9_11

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics