Skip to main content

Toward a New Paradigm of Psychology

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Spirituality and Indian Psychology

Part of the book series: International and Cultural Psychology ((ICUP))

  • 1058 Accesses

Abstract

Watson (1913) noted that “Psychology, as the behaviorist views it, is a purely objective, experimental branch of natural science, which needs introspection as little as do the sciences of chemistry and physics. It is granted that the behavior of animals can be investigated without appeal to consciousness. …The position is taken here that the behavior of man and the behavior of animals must be considered on the same plane; as being equally essential to a general understanding of behavior. It can dispense with consciousness in a psychological sense. The separate observation of ‘states of consciousness’ is, on this assumption, no more a part of the task of the psychologist than of the physicist. We might call this the return of a nonreflective and naïve use of consciousness. In this sense, consciousness may be said to be the instrument or tool with which all scientists work. Whether or not the tool is properly used at present by scientists is a problem for philosophy and not for psychology (p. 176).” We see the foundation of separation of psychology and philosophy being laid in such assertions by established psychologists of those days. Watson (1913) worked hard to make psychology a science like other sciences, which can be seen in the following quote: “This suggested elimination of states of consciousness as proper objects of investigation in themselves will remove the barrier from psychology, which exists between it and the other sciences. The findings of psychology become the functional correlates of structure and lend themselves to explanation in physico-chemical terms (p. 177).” Thus, the journey for psychology to go away from philosophy and to become a part of natural science began during the turn of the last century, and scholars were willing to go to the extent of eliminating consciousness and cognition from psychology, which is now trying to find its way back with marginal success.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    To me nididhyAsana means translating the learned ideas into practice. Ramana Maharshi encouraged his disciples to live in the world and apply this three-pronged process. So it is not about getting out of the society and meditating deeply about our true form (i.e., we are Atman), but to live in the world and practice every moment being aware that we are Atman (Osborne, 1970). abhyAs (or practice) and vairAgya (or detachment) are the two additional practices added to this three pronged introspective tool as noted in verse 6.35 of the bhagavadgItA.

  2. 2.

    Western scholars often differentiate between morality and ethics, whereas in the Indian context, dharma covers both [Chaitanya, Personal communication (2009)].

  3. 3.

    Both Vijayan and Anand do not think that “polishing” captures the spirit of what I am saying. Scriptures are known for offering their teaching in sutra (or aphorism) form where the beginning and end states are mentioned and the details of the process omitted. Often a commentator or bhASyakAr explicates the process sometimes by bringing other constructs and, at other times, by giving examples. What is being done in the polished model is similar to what a bhASyakAr does. For example, I explain how goal setting, a well-established psychological construct in Western psychology, mediates desires and anger. Anand thinks it may be better to call it an explicated model [Chandrasekar, Personal Communication (2009)]. Vijayan Munusamy [Personal Communication (2009)], on the other hand, noted that what is being done under the label of “polished to fit” is akin to what is referred to as “theoretical sensitivity” in grounded theory when a researcher uses his or her personal and temperamental bent as well as theoretical insights to create a theory that fits the data (Glaser & Strauss, 1967, pp. 46–47). I think both perspectives together capture the process of polishing the model extracted from the scripture as the researcher is being a bhASyakAr and in being that brings his or her theoretical sensitivity to the process of polishing the extracted model.

  4. 4.

    There is some discussion in the literature about whether or not smRtivibhrama and smRtibhramZa are two concepts, especially since Adi zankara did not interpret them as two concepts in his commentary on the bhagavadgItA. I take the position that they are two different concepts; smRtivibhrama originates from the root bhramati meaning wander, whereas smRtibhramZa is derived from the root bhras meaning destruction. Thus, smRtivibhrama means restlessness or unsteadiness of memory, or simply one is disarranged, bewildered, perplexed, or confused. On the other hand, smRtibhramZa means decline or decay of memory, or simply ruined memory.

  5. 5.

    A question can be raised if fulfillment of goals invariably leads to greed, which is negative. What about ambition to excel in something or non-selfish goals like desire to help others? It is my understanding that all desires invariably lead to greed, since enlightenment means flowing with the universe and serving people without having any desire for oneself. The moment there is self, there is desire and, therefore, greed. The selfless person has no desire. The enlightened person has no desire, not even to fight for the freedom of nation. One would think that the pastors and ministers of Christian churches are motivated by serving others, but research evidence shows that they have one of the highest rates of burnout among all professions (Chun,2006; Grosch & Olsen, 2000). Clearly, too much of social service can also lead to stress and burnout. Other examples can be found in the scriptures. For example, arjuna’s desire to be the best archer led him to complain to guru droNa, which culminated in ekalavya losing his thumb. The contest between karNa and arjuna was a result of both trying to be the best archer of their time. So, excellence and ambition inherently lead to competition and result in greed sooner or later.

  6. 6.

    Verse 3.14: annAdbhavanti bhUtAni parjanyAdannasambhavaH; yajnAdbhavati parjanyo yajnaH karmasamudbhavaH.

  7. 7.

    Verse 3.15: karma brahmodbhavaM viddhi brahmAkSarsamudbhavam; tasmAtsarvagataM brahma nityaM yajne pratiSThitam.

  8. 8.

    A friend noted if this entire argument or model should find a place in the paper. He thought that while what kRSNa said must have made perfect sense to the people of his time, concepts like yajnAd bhavati parjanyaH are difficult for the modern mind to accept. I thought similarly, so I can relate to the argument. During my sabbatical in New Zealand in October 2002, I was studying the bhagavadgItA, and one day I thought that yajna could not cause rain. And that same day, a visiting anthropologist happened to present about Native Americans’ practice of going in solitude to pray for rain and doing what is referred to as rain dance. After his presentation, I asked him if he believed the shamans predicted rain or made rain, and he responded that what he believed did not matter. The Native Americans believed that the shamans made rain. I stopped questioning the connection between rain and yajna from that point on. More recently in October 2009, I was reading the biography of Sri Ramana Maharshi (Osborne, 1970), and I came across a story that supports such mystical connection between yajna and rain. “The mystery of Arunachala Hill also has become more accessible. There were many formerly who felt nothing of its power, for whom it was just a hill of rock and earth and shrubs like any other. Mrs. Taleyarkhan, a devotee mentioned in the previous chapter, was sitting once on the hill with a guest of hers, talking about Sri Bhagavan. She said: “Bhagavan is a walking God and all our prayers are answered. That is my experience. Bhagavan says this hill is God Himself. I cannot understand all that, but Bhagavan says so, so I believe it.” Her friend, a Muslim in whom the courtly Persian traditions of culture still lingered, replied, “According to our Persian beliefs I would take it as a sign if it rained.” Almost immediately there was a shower and they came down the hill drenched to tell the story (Osborne, 1970, p. 192).” Thus, I am open to the correlation between prayer or yajna and rain.

  9. 9.

    agnau prAstAhutiH samyagAdityamupatiZThate; AdityAjjAyate vRSTirvRSTerannaM tataH prajAH (Manusmriti 3.76). The offering given properly to fire is placed in Sun; Sun causes rain, rain causes grains, and from grains come people.

  10. 10.

    It is plausible that actions dedicated to God and niSkAma karma are not the same. In the early phase of cultivating niSkAma karma, I remember dedicating my work to God, as if to convince myself that I was not the doer and I was not concerned about the fruits. In hindsight, actions dedicated to God are a far cry from doing niSkAma karma. niSkAma karma is similar to carrying out the will of God and knowing that it is not one’s own desire to perform the action. In the early phases of sAdhanA (or spiritual practice), sometimes we rationalize our desires as God’s will.

  11. 11.

    All personality theories listed in Wikipedia are by Western scholars, namely, Sigmund Freud, Alfred Adler, Carl Jung, Gordon Allport, B.F. Skinner, Raymond Cattell, Hans Eysenck, George Kelly, Abraham Maslow, Carl Rogers, Lewis Goldberg, John Holland, Heinz Kohut, Karen Horney, Meyer Friedman, and Richard Herrnstien among others. This provides face validity that personality is a Western construct. When people use terms like Islamic Personality or Buddhist Personality they are simply using a pseudoetic approach to study human psychology using the construct of personality.

  12. 12.

    The more the non-prime numbers are involved, the smaller is the GCF. Since prime numbers cannot be factored, the GCF for prime numbers is simply a multiplication of all the prime numbers in the set.

References

  • Adair, J. G. (1996). The indigenous psychology bandwagon: Cautions and considerations. In J. Pandey, D. Sinha, and D. P. S. Bhawuk (Eds.), Asian contributions to cross-cultural psychology (pp. 50–58). New Delhi/Thousand Oaks/London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bhawuk, D. P. S. (1997). Leadership through Relationship Management: Using the Theory of Individualism and Collectivism. In R. W. Brislin & K. Cushner (Eds.), Improving intercultural interactions: Modules for cross-cultural training programs, Volume 2, pp. 40–56. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bhawuk, D. P. S. (1999). Who Attains Peace: An Indian Model of Personal Harmony. Indian Psychological Review, 52 (2 & 3), 40–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bhawuk, D. P. S. (2001a). Looking for the Silver Lining: Integrative Potentials in International Project Negotiation. Psychology and Developing Societies, 13 (2), 243–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bhawuk, D. P. S. (2003a). Culture’s Influence on Creativity: The Case of Indian Spirituality. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 27 (1), 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bhawuk, D. P. S. (2008a). Science of Culture and Culture of Science: Worldview and Choice of Conceptual Models and Methodology. The Social Engineer, 11 (2), 26–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bhawuk, D. P. S. (2008b). Globalization and Indigenous Cultures: Homogenization or Differentiation? International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 32 (4), 305–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bhawuk, D. P. S. (2008c) Anchoring Cognition, Emotion, and Behavior in Desire: A Model from the Gita.” In K. R. Rao, A. C. Paranjpe, & A. K. Dalal (Eds.), Handbook of Indian Psychology (pp. 390–413). New Delhi, India: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bhawuk, D. P. S. (2009). Intercultural Training for the Global Workplace: Review, Synthesis, and Theoretical Explorations. In Rabi S. Bhagat & R. Steers (Eds.), Handbook of Culture, Organization, and Work (pp. 462–488). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bhawuk, D. P. S. (2010a). Methodology for building psychological models from scriptures: Contributions of Indian psychology to indigenous and global psychologies. Psychology and Developing Societies, 22 (1), 49–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bhawuk, D. P. S., Mrazek, S., & Munusamy, V. P. (2009). From social engineering to community transformation: Amul, Grameen Bank, and Mondragon as exemplar organizations. Peace & Policy :Ethical Transformations for a Sustainable Future, vol. 14, 36–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chun, D. (November 18, 2006). Pastors often succumb to job burnout due to stress, low pay. The Honolulu Advertiser, p. B3.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, Attitude, Intention, and Behavior: An Introduction to Theory and Research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley

    Google Scholar 

  • Geertz, C. J. (1973). Thick description: Toward an interpretive theory of culture. In The interpretation of cultures: selected essays (pp. 3–30). New-York, NY: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glaser, B. G., & Strauss A. L. (1967). The discovery of grounded theory: Strategies for qualitative research. New York, NY: Aldine De Gruytner.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grosch, W. N., & Olsen, D. C. (2000). Clergy burnout. An integrative approach. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 56 (5), 619–632.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hasegawa, H. (1995). A sociotypological approach to Japan’s economic development. The International Executive, 37(5), 437–449.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hwang, K. K. (2006). Constructive realism and Confucian relationalism: An epistemological strategy for the development of indigenous psychology. In U. Kim, K-S Yang, & K-K Hwang (Eds.), Indigenous and cultural psychology: Understanding people in context (pp. 73–107). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Marsella, A. J. (1998). Toward a “Global-Community Psychology,” American Psychologist, 53 (12), 1282–1291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, Charles A. (1967). Introduction: The comprehensive Indian mind. In Charles A. Moore (ed.) The Indian mind: Essentials of Indian philosophy and culture (pp. 1–18). Hawaii, Honolulu: University of Hawaii press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Munusamy, V. P. (2008). Decoding the meaning of multiculturalism: An international study of Malaysia, Singapore, and Hawaii. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Hawaii at Manoa.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paranjpe, A. C. (1984). Theoretical psychology: The meeting of East and West. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paranjpe, A. C. (1998). Self and identity in modern psychology and Indian thought. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poortinga, Y. H. (1996). Indigenous psychology: Scientific ethnocentrism in a new guise? In J. Pandey, D. Sinha, and S. P. S. Bhawuk (Eds.), Asian contributions to cross-cultural psychology (pp. 59–71). New Delhi/Thousand Oaks/London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raina, M. K. (Ed.) (1980) Creativity research: International Perspective. New Delhi, India: National Council for Educational Research and Training.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ratner, C. (2006). Cultural psychology: A perspective on psychological functioning and social reform. New York: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheldon, W. H. (1951). Main contrasts between Eastern and Western philosophy. In Charles A. Moore, (Ed.) Essays in East-West Philosophy (pp. 288–297). Honolulu, University of Hawaii Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shweder, R. A. (1990). Cultural psychology—what is it? In J. W. Stigler, R. A. Shweder, & G. Herdt (Eds.), Cultural psychology: Essays on comparative human development (pp. 1–43). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Stake, R. E. (2005). Qualitative case studies. In N. K. Denzin & Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.), The Sage Handbook of qualitative research (3rd ed., pp. 443–466). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Triandis, Harry, C. (1979). Values, attitudes, and interpersonal behavior, in Howe, H.E. and Page, M.M. (Eds.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Lincoln, NE: Nebraska University Press, 196–259.

    Google Scholar 

  • Triandis, H. C. (1994a). Cross-cultural industrial and organizational psychology. In H. C. Triandis, M. D. Dunnette, & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (2nd edition) (Vol. 4, pp. 103–172). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Triandis, H. C. (1994b). Culture and social behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B. (1913). Psychology as the behaviorist views it. Psychological Review, 20, 158–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Dharm P. S. Bhawuk .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2011 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Bhawuk, D.P.S. (2011). Toward a New Paradigm of Psychology. In: Spirituality and Indian Psychology. International and Cultural Psychology. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-8110-3_10

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics