Skip to main content
Log in

Time to understand pictures and words

  • Letter
  • Published:

From Nature

View current issue Submit your manuscript

Abstract

WHEN an object such as a chair is presented visually, or is represented by a line drawing, a spoken word, or a written word, the initial stages in the process leading to understanding are clearly different in each case. There is disagreement, however, about whether those early stages lead to a common abstract representation in memory, the idea of a chair1–4, or to two separate representations, one verbal (common to spoken and written words), and the other image-like5. The first view claims that words and images are associated with ideas, but the underlying representation of an idea is abstract. According to the second view, the verbal representation alone is directly associated with abstract information about an object (for example, its superordinate category: furniture). Concrete perceptual information (for example, characteristic shape, colour or size) is associated with the imaginal representation. Translation from one representation to the other takes time, on the second view, which accounts for the observation that naming a line drawing takes longer than naming (reading aloud) a written word6,7. Here we confirm that naming a drawing of an object takes much longer than reading its name, but we show that deciding whether the object is in a given category such as ‘furniture’ takes slightly less time for a drawing than for a word, a result that seems to be inconsistent with the second view.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Schank, R. C., Cog. Psychol., 3, 552–631 (1972).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Anderson, J. R., and Bower, G. H., Human Associative Memory (Wiley, New York, 1973).

    Google Scholar 

  3. Seymour, P. H. K., Br. J. Psychol., 64, 35–49 (1973).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Potter, M. C., Science (in the press).

  5. Paivio, A., Imagery and Verbal Processes (Holt, Rinehart and Winston, New York 1971).

    Google Scholar 

  6. Woodworth, R. S., Experimental Psychology, (Holt, New York, 1938).

    Google Scholar 

  7. Fraisse, P., Percept. mot. Skills, 11, 204 (1960); Annee Psychol., 64, 21–46 (1964).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Wingfield, A., Acta. Psychol., 26, 216–226 (1967); Am. J. Psychol., 81, 226–234 (1968).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Cohen, G., Q. J. exp. Psychol., 25, 557–564 (1973).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Forster, R. I., and Chambers, S. M., J. verbal Learning verbal Behav., 12, 627–635 (1973).

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

POTTER, M., FAULCONER, B. Time to understand pictures and words. Nature 253, 437–438 (1975). https://doi.org/10.1038/253437a0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/253437a0

  • Springer Nature Limited

This article is cited by

Navigation