Skip to main content
Log in

Who Are Tomboys and Why Should We Study Them?

  • Published:
Archives of Sexual Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Tomboys are girls who behave like boys and, as such, challenge some theories of sex-typing. We recruited tomboys (N = 60) ages 4–9 through the media and compared them with their sisters (N = 15) and brothers (N = 20) on measures of playmate preference, sex-typed activities and interests, and gender identity. On nearly all measures, tomboys were substantially and significantly more masculine than their sisters, but they were generally less masculine than their brothers. We outline some scientific benefits of studying tomboys and describe some goals and initial findings of the Tomboy Project.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Bailey, J. M., & Zucker, K. J. (1995). Childhood sex-typed behavior and sexual orientation: A conceptual analysis and quantitative review. Developmental Psychology, 31, 43-55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bechtold, K. T. (1996). Sex-segregation: Is it due to gender identity or toy and activity preferences? Unpublished master's thesis, Southern Illinois University, Carbondale.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berenbaum, S. A. (1999). Effects of early androgens on sex-typed activities and interests in adolescents with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Hormones and Behavior, 35, 102-110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berenbaum, S. A., & Bailey, J. M. (1998). Variation in female gender identity: Evidence from girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia, tomboys, and typical girls. Unpublished manuscript.

  • Berenbaum, S. A., Bryk, K., Levine, S., Huttenlocher, J., & Bailey, J. M. (1999, April). Associations between sex-typed play and spatial abilities in tomboys and typical girls. Poster presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Albuquerque, NM.

  • Berenbaum, S. A., & Hines, M. (1992). Early androgens are related to childhood sex-typed toy preferences. Psychological Science, 3, 203-206.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berenbaum, S. A., & Resnick, S. M. (1997). Early androgen effects on aggression in children and adults with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 22, 505-515.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berenbaum, S. A., & Snyder, E. (1995). Early hormonal influences on childhood sex-typed activity and playmate preferences: Implications for the development of sexual orientation. Developmental Psychology, 31, 31-42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bussey, K., & Bandura, A. (1999). Social cognitive theory of gender development and differentiation. Psychological Review, 106, 676-713.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caspi, A., & Bem, D. J. (1990). Personality continuity and change across the life course. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 549-575). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conley, J. J. (1984). Longitudinal consistency of adult personality: Selfreported psychological characteristics across 45 years. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 1325-1333.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conley, J. J. (1985). Longitudinal stability of personality traits: A multitrait-multimethod-multioccasion analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1266-1282.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fridell, S. R., Zucker, K. J., Bradley, S. J., & Maing, D. M. (1996). Physical attractivenss of girls with gender identity disorder. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 25, 17-31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goy, R.W., Bercovitch, F. B., & McBrair, M. C. (1988). Behavioral maculinization is independent of genital masculinization in prenatally androgenized female rhesus macaques. Hormones and Behavior, 22, 552-571.

    Google Scholar 

  • Green, R. (1987). The “sissy boy syndrome” and the development of homosexuality. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Green, R., Williams, K., & Goodman, M. (1982). Ninety-nine “tomboys” and “non-tomboys”: Behavioral contrasts and demographic similarities. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 11, 247-266.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huston, A. C. (1983). Sex-typing. In E. M. Hetherington (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Socialization, personality, and social development (Vol. 4, pp. 388-467). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hyde, J. S., Rosenberg, B. G., & Behrman, J. A. (1977). Tomboyism. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 2, 73-75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laumann, E. O., Gagnon, J. H., Michael, R. T., & Michaels, S. (1994). The social organization of sexuality: Sexual practices in the United States. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Linn, M. C., & Peterson, A. C. (1985). Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 56, 1479-1498.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lippa, R. (1995). Do sex differences define gender-related individual differences within the sexes? Evidence from three studies. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 349-355.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lippa, R., & Connelly, S. (1990). Gender diagnosticity: A new Bayesian approach to gender-related individual differences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1051-1065.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liss, M. B. (1981). Patterns of toy play: An analysis of sex differences. Sex Roles, 7, 1143-1150.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maccoby, E. E. (1998). The two sexes: Growing up apart, coming together. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L. (1990). Attitudes and expectations about children with nontraditional and traditional gender roles. Sex Roles, 22, 151-165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L. (1995). Stereotypes about children with traditional and nontraditional gender roles. Sex Roles, 33, 727-751.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., & Halverson, C. F. (1981). A schematic processing model of sex typing and stereotyping in children. Child Development, 52, 1119-1134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merriam-Webster (Ed.). (1998). Merriam-Webster's collegiate dictionary (10th ed.). Springfield, MA: Merriam-Webster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F. L., Feldman, J. F., & Ehrhardt, A. A. (1985). Questionnaires for the assessment of atypical gender role behavior: Amethodological study. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 24, 695-701.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, B. L. (1998). A three generational study of tomboy behavior. Sex Roles, 39, 787-800.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plumb, P., & Cowan, G. (1984). Adevelopmental study of destereotyping and androgynous activity preferences of tomboys, nontomboys, and males. Sex Roles, 10, 703-712.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reinisch, J. M. (1981). Prenatal exposure to synthetic progestins increases potential for aggression in humans. Science, 211, 1171-1173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reinisch, J. M., & Sanders, S. A. (1986). A test of sex differences in aggressive response to hypothetical conflict situations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 1045-1049.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruble, D. N., & Martin, C. L. (1998). Gender development. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (5th ed., pp. 933-1016). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sandberg, D. E., & Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F. L. (1994). Variability in middle childhood play behavior: Effects of gender, age, and family background. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 23, 645-663.

    Google Scholar 

  • Serbin, L. A., Zelkowitz, P., Doyle, A., Gold, D., & Wheaton, B. (1990). The socialization of sex-differentiated skills and academic performance: A mediational model. Sex Roles, 23, 613-628.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, J. L. (1999). Stability and change in vocational interests. In M. Savickas & A. Spokane (Eds.), Vocational interests: Meaning, measurement and counseling use (pp. 135-158). Palo Alto, CA: Davies-Black.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorne, B. (1993). Gender play: Girls and boys in school. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Udry, J. R. (2000). Biological limites of gender construction. American Sociological Review, 65, 443-457.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, K., Goodman, M., & Green, R. (1985). Parent-child factors in gender role socialization in girls. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 26, 720-731.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, K., Green, R., & Goodman, M. (1979). Patterns of sexual identity development: A preliminary report on the “tomboy.” Research in Community and Mental Health, 1, 103-123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zucker, K. J., & Bradley, S. J. (1995). Gender identity disorder and psychosexual problems in children and adolescents. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zucker, K. J., Bradley, S. J., Sullivan, C. L., Kuksis, M., Birkenfeld-Adams, A., & Mitchell, J. N. (1993). A gender identity interview for children. Journal of Personality Assessment, 61, 443-456.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zuger, B. (1988). Is early effeminate behavior in boys early homosexuality? Comprehensive Psychiatry, 29, 509-519.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bailey, J.M., Bechtold, K.T. & Berenbaum, S.A. Who Are Tomboys and Why Should We Study Them?. Arch Sex Behav 31, 333–341 (2002). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1016272209463

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1016272209463

Navigation