Skip to main content
Log in

Effects of Level of Processing on Implicit and Explicit Memory in Depressed Mood

  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Recently, several studies have addressed the question of whether depression affects priming in implicit memory tasks. The main aim of this experiment was to assess the presence of a bias for negative information in explicit memory (free recall) and implicit memory (word-stem completion) tasks among subclinically depressed subjects compared to nondepressed subjects, using the typical levels of processing manipulation. The results of this study show the existence of a mood-congruent memory bias for both implicit and explicit memory in depressed subjects. The theoretical implications of these findings for implicit and explicit memory biases associated with depressed mood are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561-571.

    Google Scholar 

  • Besson, M., Fischler, I., Boaz, T., & Raney, G. (1992). Effects of automatic associative activation on explicit and implicit memory tests. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 18, 89-105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blaney, P. H. (1986). Affect and memory: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 229-246.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129-148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G. H. (1987). commentary on mood and memory. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 25, 443-455.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, B., & Mathews, A. (1983). Negative self-schemata in clinical depression. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 22, 173-181.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., & Williams, R. (1994). Implicit and explicit memory for emotional information in non-clinical subjects. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32, 65-78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., & Millar, N. (1996). Evidence of emotion-congruent implicit memory effects in clinical and non-cinical depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34, 865-879.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., & Williams, R. (1995). Implicit and explicit memory for emotion-congruent information in clinical depression and anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 755-770.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A. S., & Mitchell, D. B. (1994). A reevaluation of semantic versus nonsemantic processing in implicit memory. Memory and Cognition, 22, 533-541.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bumberry, W., Oliver, J., & McClure, J. N. (1978). Validation of the Beck Depression Inventory in a university population using psychiatric estimate as the criterion. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46, 150-155.

    Google Scholar 

  • Challis, B. H., & Brodbeck, D. R. (1992). Level of processing affects priming in word fragment completion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 18, 595-607.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, D. M., & Teasdale, J. D. (1985). Constraints on the effects of mood on memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1595-1608.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cloitre, M., Shear, M. K., Cancienne, J., & Zeitlin, S. B. (1994). Implicit and explicit memory for catastrophic associations to bodily sensation words in panic disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 18, 225-240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conde, V., Esteban, T., & Useros, E. (1976). Revisión crítica de la adaptación castellana del cuestionario de Beck [A critical review of the Spanish translation of Beck's scale]. Revista de Psicología General y Aplicada, 31, 369-497.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craik, F. I. M., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing:A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 671-684.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craik, F. I. M., & Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and the relation of words in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 268-294.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dalgleish, T., & Watts, F. N. (1990). Biases of attention and memory in disorders of anxiety and depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 10, 589-604.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denny, E. B., & Hunt, R. R. (1992). Affective valence and memory in depression: Dissociation of recall and fragment completion. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 575-580.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dorfman, J., & Mandler, G. (1994). Implicit and explicit forgetting: When is gist remebered? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47A, 651-672.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, J. C., & Kirsner, K. (1988). Discovering functionally independent mental processes: The principle of reversed association. Psychological Review, 95, 91-101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliot, C. L., & Greene, R. B. (1992). Clinical depression and implicit memory. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 572-574.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilligan, C. G., & Bower, G. H. (1984). Cognitive consequences of emotional arousal. In C. E. Izard, J. Kagan, & R. B. Zajonc, (eds.), Emotions, cognition and behavior (pp. 547-588). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graf, P., & Mandler, G. (1984). Activation makes words more accessible, but not necessarily more retrievable. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 553-568.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graf, P., Mandler, G., & Haden, P. (1982). Simulating amnesic symptoms in normal subjects. Science, 218, 1234-1244.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graf, P., & Schacter, D. L. (1985). Implicit and explicit memory for new associations in normal and amnesic subjects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 11, 501-518.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greene, R. L. (1990). Spacing effects on implicit memory test. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 1004-1011.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hertel, P. T. (1994). Depressive deficits in word identification and recall. Cognition and Emotion, 8, 313-327.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hertel, P. T., & Hardin, T. S. (1990). Remembering with and without awareness in depressed mood. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119, 45-59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby, L. L. (1983). Perceptual enhancement: Persistent effects of an experience. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 9, 21-38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby, L. L. (1991). A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic and intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 513-541.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby, L. L., & Dallas, M. (1981). On the relationship between autobiographical memory and perceptual learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 306-340.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby, L. L., Toth, J. P., & Yonelinas, A. P. (1993). Separating conscious and unconscious influences of memory: Measuring recollection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 139-154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Juilland, A., & Chang-Rodríguez, E. (1964). Frequency dictionary of Spanish words. The Hague, Netherlands: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendall, P. C., & Watson, D. (1989). Anxiety and depression. Distinctive and overlapping features. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C. (1990). Mood disorders and cognition. In M. W. Eysenck (Ed.), Cognitive psychology: An international review. Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G. (1980). Recognizing: The judgment of previous occurrence. Psychological Review, 87, 252-271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G. (1991). Your face looks familiar but I can't remember your name: A review of dual process theory. In I. W. E. Hockley & S. Lewandowsky (Eds.), Relating theory and data: Essays on human memory in honour of Bennet B. Murdock (pp. 207-225). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, A., Mogg, K., May, J., & Eysenck, M. (1989). Implicit and explicit memory bias in anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98, 236-240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matt, G. E., Vázquez, C., & Campbell, W. K. (1992). Mood-congruent recall of affectively toned stimuli: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 12, 227-255.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDowall, J. (1984). Recall of pleasant and unpleasant words in depressed subjects. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 93, 401-407.

    Google Scholar 

  • Polaino, A., Vázquez, C., & Ochoa, E. F L. (1983). An epidemiological study of the prevalence of depression in a sample of the University of Madrid. Unpublished manuscript, Universidad Compultense, Madrid.

  • Reingold, E. M., & Merikle, P. M. (1990). On the inter-relatedness of theory and measurement in the study of unconscious processes. Mind and Language, 5, 9-28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richard, A. & French, C. C. (1991). Effects of encoding and anxiety on implicit and explicit memory performance. Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 131-139.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richardson-Klavehn, A., & Bjork, R. A. (1988). Measures of memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 39, 475-543.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richardson-Klavehn, A., Gardiner, J. M., & Java, R. J. (1994). Involuntary conscious memory and the method of opposition. Memory, 2, 1-29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., III, & McDermott, K. B. (1992). Depression and implicit memory: A commentary. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 587-591.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., III, & McDermott, K. B. (1993). Implicit memory in normal human subjects. In F. Boller & J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology (Vol. 8, pp. 63-131). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., III, Weldon, M. S., Stadler, M. L., & Riegler, G. L. (1992). Direct comparison of two implicit memory tests: Word fragment and word stem completion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 1251-1269.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruiz-Caballero, J. A., Bermúdez, J. (1993). The role of affective focus: Replication and extension of mood congruent and memory. Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 191-197.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruiz-Caballero, J. A., & González, P. (1994). Implicit and explicit memory bias in depressed and nondepressed subjects. Cognition and Emotion, 8, 555-570.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, D. L., Gerard, L., & Cortese, C. (1979). Accessing lexical memory: The transfer of word repetition effects across task and modality. Memory and Cognition, 7, 3-12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L. (1987). Implicit memory: History and current status. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 13, 501-518.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L., & Graf, P. (1986). Effects of elaborative processing on implicit and explicit memory for new associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 12, 432-444.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singer, J. A., & Salovey, P. (1988). Mood and memory: Evaluating the network theory of affect. Clinical Psychology Review, 8, 211-251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teasdale, J. D., & Russell, M. L. (1983) Differential effects of induced mood on recall of positive, negative and neutral words. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 22, 163-171.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thapar, A., & Greene, R. L. (1994). Effects of level of processing on implicit and explicit tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 20, 671-679.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toth, J. P., Lindsay, D. S., & Jacoby, L. L. (1992). Awareness, automaticity, and memory dissociations. In L. R. Squire & N. Butters (Eds.), Neuropsychology of memory (2nd ed., pp. 46-57). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toth, J. P., Reingold, E. M., & Jacoby, L. L. (1994). Toward a redefinition of implicit memory: Process dissociations following elaborative processing and self-generation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 20, 290-303.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E., Schacter, D. L., & Stark, H. A. (1982). Priming effects in word-fragment completion are independent of recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 8, 336-342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, P. C., Mathews, A., Williamson, D. A., & Fuller, R. D. (1992). Mood-congruent memory in depression: Emotional priming or elaboration? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 581-586.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. M. G., Watts, F. N., MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (1988). Cognitive psychology and the emotional disorders. Chichester, England: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeitlin, S. B., & McNally R. J. (1991). Implicit and explicit memory bias for threat in post-traumatic stress disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 29, 451-457.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Ruiz-Caballero, J.A., González, P. Effects of Level of Processing on Implicit and Explicit Memory in Depressed Mood. Motivation and Emotion 21, 195–209 (1997). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1024438701205

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1024438701205

Keywords

Navigation