Introduction

One of the consequences of the information age is that people increasingly rely on computers to do a variety of everyday jobs, like, reading information through web-pages, processing data, writing report, sending e-mail. The visual display terminal (VDT) is the primary medium through which humans interact. Studies report readability performance taking into consideration various factors individually or in combination, taking two factors at a time (Legge et al. 1985, 1987). However, combining three or more factors together to assess the influence on operator performance has not been substantially addressed. We used a powerful optimization tool called the Taguchi method for analysis, combining three operating parameters of human computer system to assess the readability performance of operators. Many operating parameters may be combined together but complications in deriving true results out of all these factors restrict one to consider few parameters and also few levels at a time. The Taguchi method is a powerful tool for optimizing design as well as operating parameters. In Ergonomics/Human Factors, use of this optimization tool is still in infancy. Use of this optimization tool will probably obtain better results. The numerous studies pertaining to application of optimization in different areas are available in the literature (Bashiri et al. 2013; Mariajayaprakash et al. 2013; Bashiri and Moslemi 2013; Ghasemi et al. 2013; Shokuhfar et al. 2008; Farsani et al. 2001).

The Taguchi method is a well-known technique that provides a systematic and efficient methodology for process/design optimization. It has been widely used for product design and process optimization worldwide (Wang and Tarng 1998; Chung-Chen and Hong 2002; Nian et al. 1999). This is due to the advantages of the design of experiment using Taguchi’s technique, which includes simplification of experimental plan and feasibility of study of interaction between different parameters. Lesser number of experiments means time and costs are reduced. Taguchi proposes experimental plan in terms of orthogonal array that gives different combinations of parameters and their levels for each experiment. According to this technique, the entire parameter space is studied with minimal number of necessary experiments (Roy 2001; Phadke 1989). Based on the average output value at each parameter level, main effect analysis is performed. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) is then used to determine which process parameter is statistically significant and the contribution of each process parameter toward the output characteristics. With the main effect and ANOVA analysis, possible combination of optimum parameters can be predicted. Finally, a confirmation experiment is conducted to verify the optimal process parameters obtained from the process parameter design.

Numerous reports say that continuous use of computer causes occupational problems and high level of discomfort to human eyes (Grandjean 1980; Zhu and Wu 1990). The development in information system design suggests that visual display units (VDUs) will continue to be used as a medium for presentation of large amount of connected texts as in videotext, electronic journals, dynamic books or such similar applications. With the passage of time the user interaction with their machines is increasing. This exposure to VDUs may bring in unheard kind and extent of effects that might lead to damage or impairment of human health. Various parameters may apparently influence the human–computer interaction (HCI) environment, working postures, distance from the VDU, level of illumination in the workplace, color contrast of the background and the projected matter, environmental variables like, noise pollution, temperature, humidity etc. The readability performance of operator is significantly dependant on text/background color combination. Sanders and McCormic (1993) have made recommendations regarding the best screen text/background color combinations including, “avoid using the extremes of the color spectrum, the reds and blues” or “use as few colors as possible”. Shieh and Chen (1997) reported that subjects’ viewing distance was significantly affected by color combination. Similarly, Wang et al. (2002) also reported that subjects’ searching performance on leading display was improving when the color difference of text/background becomes larger. Wang and Chen (2003) reported recently that the interaction between jump length and color combination of leading display had a significant effect on subjects’ reading performance.

It is reported that the background should not be ‘emphasized’ in screen components, however, a higher contrast should be reserved for screen components to which attention should be drawn (Galitz 1997). Research findings are mixed, but still text/background color combination is an important consideration in VDT design. Some studies have claimed that there is no effect of text/background color combination on readability performance while others reported improvement in the performance with dark characters on a light background (Wang et al. 2002; Taptagaporn and Saito 1990; Snyderet al. 1990). Shieh and Lin (2000), indicated that visual preference increased with increase in luminance contrast in color combination of text or background. The viewing angle is also an important factor in readability. Leavitt (1995) reported that eye strain symptoms may occur in up to 75 % of all VDT users. Sanders and McCormic (1993) recommended to place the center of the monitor approximately 15° below horizontal eye level because it allows the operator to assume a comfortable and natural posture. Other researchers reported that blinking rate of an eye is a reliable indicator of dry eyes and visual discomfort (Villanueva et al. 1996). On the other hand, Ankrum and Nemeth (1995) postulated that the user will feel more comfortable while viewing monitor at 40° thereby reducing static loading of the muscles of the neck, shoulder and back. Turville et al. (1998) investigated the 40° VDT position and found that it caused greater head tilt angles and higher muscle activity levels for six of the 10 necks. The third variable, viewing distance, is also a critical factor from operators’ readability view point. Users must be able to select their preferred viewing distance when using a VDT. Jaschinski-Kruza (1991) showed that healthy subjects, when free to set their own viewing distance, choose distance between 51 and 99 cm for characters 5 mm or high. Szeto and Lee (2002) reported a viewing distance of 53.6 ± 7.4 cm for desktop computer while making assessment for VDT workstation. Shieh and Chen (1997) found that screen color combination significantly affected viewing distance, with red text on a green background resulting in a shorter viewing distance and a greater standard deviation of viewing distance than other color combination. Also Shieh (2002) found that the mean viewing distance for LCD was 42.3 cm and viewing distance correlated significantly with subjective visual fatigue. Knoblauch et al. (1991) measured reading rates for texts when character sizes were between 0.15° and 6°. The highest reading rates were found for high luminance contrast and character sizes between 0.2° and 1°. Although researches in this particular area are being conducted taking into consideration different factors, a need to optimize operator performance from readability view point taking three factors together was felt and experimental investigation in this case was carried out taking appropriate factors and their levels.

Method

Subjects

32 Subjects (16 males and 16 females) were recruited from Universiti Sains Malaysia. They were right handed and with a mean age of 25 year (SD = ±8.25), mean weight 75.8 kg (SD = ±17.2). All participants could read Malay fluently. All had 0.8 correct visual acuity or better and normal color vision. Most participants (90 %) reported reading from computer regularly.

Apparatus and VDT workplace condition

The experiment was conducted at an adjustable VDT workstation. Text was displayed on a Pentium IV computer, using a 60 Hz, 96 dpi 17 inch high-resolution RGB monitor. The print was high contrast (Weber contrast 0.94). The luminance contrast between the characters and background was kept nearly constant. The luminance (Y) and CIE chromaticity coordinates (x, y) of white and black for the LCD screen were measured with a Minolta analyzer CS-100. Measurements were done in similar lighting conditions. Photometric readings were measured from the center of the display for both text and background. The monitor was placed on a table 73 cm in height. The front edge of table was 35 cm from the screen center. The distance from the participants’ eyes to the screen center was 40, 50 and 60 cm respectively in different experiments and measured by an auto focusing video recorder. The inclination of monitor was 105°. Two monitor placements were used for this study, 15° and 40° below the horizontal line of sight. The workplace was illuminated by fluorescent tube. The lamps were covered with a shield and number of lamps was increased to make the ambient illuminance about 350 lux. The workplace arrangement is shown in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

The workplace arrangement and schematic diagram of experimental set-up

Task and procedure

The stimuli presented to the subjects on the LCD screen were in Malay language, written in usual Roman script, and spacing between the lines was 1 mm. The characters per screen of text were arranged in 25 lines. Three texts of required length were presented to the subjects for readability task to avoid any learning effect included in the experiment. Subjects were instructed to scroll through the stimuli by clicking the down arrow button on the key board as they read. For specific viewing distance, the letter size required was adjusted and presented to the subjects. Each combination number was repeated three times with stated parameter maintained at the required level. To familiarize with the procedure of experiment, subjects were required to perform readability task prior to the actual experiment. Each combination number was repeated three times thus consuming a total of 9 min (3 min each time) to complete one set of experiment. Subjects started reading task as they heard the instruction “START” and stopped as they heard “STOP”. Thus readability performance in number of words per minute (WPM) was obtained by selecting the total passage read divided by time taken. Table 3, shows the performance in WPM as the average of the three observations, with fraction part omitted, for 15° viewing angle while similar experiments were carried out for 40° viewing angle. Duration of experiment for all sets of 9 combinations was 81 min as each participant repeated the task 3 times in one particular setting of experiment. Juola et al. (1995) reported that speed of 260 and 171 wpm had no significant effect on subjects’ reading accuracy. Similarly, in this experiment, subjects made negligible reading error. Note that the inter-individual differences were not taken into account while determining stimulus parameters.

Selection of parameters and their levels

The readability performance experiments were carried out in the School of Mechanical Engineering, Universiti Sains Malaysia, in a computer room under above stated conditions. The initial operating parameters are based on the normal working of human–machine system. In the existing human–computer interaction conditions for subjects, their performance in terms of words per minute (WPM) were found to be 190 ± 10 WPM, without specifying the values of parameters under consideration. For determining the base line values, selected subjects were asked to perform readability task by adjusting their parameters according to their convenience and on the basis of this it was found that values of the parameters under reference were not fixed. However, luminance contrast = 0.12 ± 0.04, viewing distance = 47 ± 13 cm and character size = 1.6 ± 1.2 degree were noted. While keeping the other parameters same as for other experiments, the readability performances of the subjects were evaluated and it was found as 190 ± 10 WPM. Different values of text/background color combination, viewing distance and character size were selected after extensive literature review and surveys. For example, Legge et al. (1987) investigated the reading rates for moving lines of texts and found that maximum reading rates were achieved at character sizes of 0.3°–2° regardless of the polarity of the text. Similarly Knoblauch et al. (1991) measured reading rates for texts and found the character for which the highest reading rates were achieved increased from 0.2–1 to 2–4°. Based on the above findings, character sizes chosen for present study were 0.2°, 1.5° and 4. Wang and Chen (2003) reported the interaction between text/background color combination with jump length for Chinese typography and found that when jump length was at a higher level subjects’ reading performance for the text/background color combinations with higher color difference was significantly better than performance for the color combinations with lower color difference. Similarly, Wang et al. (2002) also reported that subjects’ searching performance on leading display was improving when the color difference of text/background becomes larger. Thus, there has been a need to discuss the effect of text/background color combination on subjects’ reading performance. Therefore, higher color combination has been chosen for this study, i.e., white-on-black, black-on-white and blue-on-white. Viewing distance adopted by the operators varied from study to study and no conclusive result has been reported. Therefore, in the present study a range of values related to viewing distance was chosen, i.e., 40, 50 and 60 cm. The selected parameter and their respective values (levels) have been shown in Table 1.

Table 1 Operating parameters and their levels

Determination of optimal operating parameters

In this section, the use of an orthogonal array to reduce the number of experiments for determining the operating parameters is reported. Results of readability performance (in number of words read per minute) experiments are analyzed by the signal-to-noise (S/N) ratio and analysis of variance (ANOVA), optimal operating parameters and their levels were determined to maximize the readability performance of the operators without error in a human–computer interaction kind of working environment. Subsequently a confirmation test was carried out to verify the optimized result.

Orthogonal array experiment

The selection of orthogonal array to use basically depends on number of factors considered in the study and interaction of interest, number of levels for the factors of interest and desired experimental resolution or cost limitations.

To select an appropriate orthogonal array for experiments, the total degrees of freedom need to be determined. The degree of freedom is defined as the number of comparisons between operating parameters to be made to determine which level is better and specifically how much better. For example, a three level process parameter counts for two degrees of freedom. In the present study, since each parameter has three levels therefore, there are six degrees of freedom due to the three parameters in the readability performance operation. The degree of freedom for the orthogonal array should be greater than or at least equal to those for operation parameters. In this study, an L9 orthogonal array is used and shown in Table 2. Detail related to orthogonal array selection for specific problem can be found in Roy (2001); Ross (1996).

Table 2 Experimental layout using an L9 orthogonal array

Data collection

Before starting the actual experiment, a trial experiment was performed to familiarize the participants with the way they will perform the experiment. Nine experiments as per the plan shown in Table 2 were performed. Each experiment was repeated three times and number of words read per minute was obtained. While calculating number of words per minute, fractional values were not considered. The overall experimental results are shown in Table 3 for 15° viewing angle (Experiment-1) and for 40° viewing angle (experiment-2). Signal-to-noise ratio measures the sensitivity of the quality investigated to those uncontrollable factors in the experiment. The higher value of S/N ratio is desirable because greater S/N ratio will result in smaller product variance around the target value. Mathematical formula to calculate S/N ratio has been given in the following section. Table 4 shows the respective S/N ratios for experiment-1 and experiment-2. Tables 5 and 6 show the S/N ratio response table for the 15° and 40° viewing angles’ experiments, respectively.

Table 3 Experimental result for readability task performance (15°)
Table 4 S/N ratio for different parameters for different viewing angles
Table 5 S/N response table for readability performance (15° viewing angle)
Table 6 S/N response table for readability performance (40° viewing angle)

Results and discussion

The Taguchi method as explained in introduction section is a powerful tool for design as well as process optimization (Wang and Tarng 1998; Chung-Chen and Hong 2002; Nian et al. 1999). The purpose of process optimization is to determine the factor levels that can generate the best performance of the process under study. In this study, use of this quality tool has been made for process optimization combining several factors together i.e. text/background color, viewing distance and character size.

The Taguchi method uses the S/N ratio to measure the deviation of quality characteristics from the desired value. In the Taguchi method, the term signal represents the desirable value (mean) for the output characteristics. Therefore, the S/N ratio, η is the ratio of the mean and Standard deviation. Taguchi uses the S/N ratio to measure the quality characteristics deviating from the desired value. The value of S/N ratio is calculated using the Eqs. (1) and (2) given below. It can be defined using the following equation:

$$ \eta = - 10{ \log }\left({\text{MSD}} \right) $$
(1)

where, MSD = mean square deviation for the output characteristics. There are three categories of performance characteristics; namely, the-lower-the-better, the-higher-the-better and the-nominal-the-better. To obtain the higher readability performances, the-higher-the-better characteristics for readability performance should be taken. The MSD for higher-the-better quality characteristics can be expressed as:

$$ {\text{MSD}} = 1/{{n}}\sum { 1/{{R}}^{ 2}} $$
(2)

where, n = number of observations and R = the average number of WPM. The S/N ratio, η for the nine experiments was calculated using the above two formulae and is shown in Table 4 for both the experiments. Since the experimental design (Table 2) is orthogonal, it is then possible to separate out the effect of each operational parameter at different levels. For example, the mean S/N ratio for text/background color at levels 1, 2 and 3 can be calculated by averaging the S/N ratio for experiment number 1–3, 4–6 and 7–9, respectively (Table 2). The mean of the multi-response S/N ratio for each level of the other operating parameters can be computed in a similar manner. The respective factors and their corresponding S/N ratio values have been shown graphically in Figs. 1 and 2 respectively. Tables 5, 6 show mean S/N ratio for each parameter at three different levels for experiment 1 and 2 respectively. From these tables the highest value for each mean S/N ratio for each operating parameters can be identified clearly. In experiment-1, text/background color at level 3 (blue-on-white), viewing distance at level 3 (60 cm) and character size at level 2 (1.5 deg.) give the highest value. On the other hand, text/background color at level 3 (black-on-white), viewing distance at level 3 (60 cm) and character size at level 3 (4°) give the highest values for experiment-2. In both the studies, the text/background color at level 3 (black-on-white) gave the optimum performance (maximum number of WPM). This result is consistent with the finding of Wang et al. (2002), Wang and Chen (2003). Similarly, in both the experiments it was found that preferable viewing distance is 60 cm. This finding is in agreement with Jaschinski-Krutz (1991; Szeto and Lee (2002); Jaschinski-Kruza (1998).Viewing distance may reflect the appropriateness of various aspects of VDT design. The character size varied from 0.2°. to 4.00° for obtaining the maximum readability performance in the two experiments conducted. This is in agreement with results obtained by Knoblauch et al. (1991); Ojanpaa and Nasanen (2003). The finding of this work in terms of preferred viewing angle adopted by the subjects to give better readability performance is 15°. Various viewing angles were suggested but it was found that subjects preferred 15° viewing angle (Turville et al. 1998). Figures 2 and 3, show the S/N graph for the experimental results. Regardless of the lower-the- better or higher-the-better performance characteristics, the larger the multi-response S/N ratio, the smaller is the variance of performance characteristics around the desired value. However, the relative percentage contribution of each parameter toward optimum performance characteristics still needs to be known so that the optimal combinations of the operating parameters and their levels can be determined more accurately.

Fig. 2
figure 2

S/N graph showing readability performance for factors A, B and C (15° viewing angle)

Fig. 3
figure 3

S/N graph showing readability performance for factors A, B and C (40° viewing angle)

Analysis of variance (ANOVA)

The purpose of carrying out ANOVA is to investigate which of the operation parameters significantly affects the performance characteristics. Tables 7 and 8 show the results of analysis of variance. Statistically, F test can be used to determine which operating parameters have significant effect on the readability performance. In the F-test, mean square (MS) due to each operating parameters needs to be calculated. The MS is equal to the sum of the squared deviations divided by the degrees of freedom associated with the operating parameters. The F value for each operating parameter is a ratio of the MS to the mean square of error. The larger the F value, the greater the effect on the performance characteristics (no of words read per minute) due to the change of operating parameters. Usually, when F > 4 it means that the change of process parameter has a significant effect on the quality characteristic (Wang and Tarng 1998). From the results of the ANOVA presented in Tables 7, 8, it can be seen that none of the process parameter is statistically significant. However, each parameter contributes to the quality characteristics (maximum number of words read per minute). The contribution order for experiment-1 is: text/background color (40.55 %), viewing distance (10.61 %) and character size (45.13 %). Similarly, the contribution order for experiment-2 is: text/background color (35.70 %), viewing distance (13.45 %) and character size (45.25 %). Therefore, based on the S/N ratio and ANOVA analyses, the optimal parameters for achieving maximum readability for 150 viewing angle (Experiment-1) in terms of number of words read per minute were at text/background color at level 3 i.e. A3 (black-on-white), viewing distance at level 3 i.e. B3 (60 cm) and character size at level 2 i.e. C2 (1.5°). The percentage contribution for each factor has been shown in Fig. 4 for experiment-1, and in Fig. 5 for experiment-2 respectively. Similarly, on the basis of S/N ratio and ANOVA analyses, the optimal parameters obtained for experiment-2, were A3B3C3. It is evident from the results that the character size contributes the most to the readability performance in experiment-1 as well as experiment-2.

Table 7 Result of the analysis of variance for readability performance (15° viewing angle)
Table 8 Result of the analysis of variance for readability performance (40° viewing angle)
Fig. 4
figure 4

Pie chart showing percentage contribution of factors A (40.55 %), B (10.61 %) and C (45.13 %) (15° viewing angle)

Fig. 5
figure 5

Pie chart showing percentage contribution of factors A (35.70 %), B (13.45 %) and C (45.25 %) (40° viewing angle)

Confirmation test

Estimated readability performance at optimum condition was calculated by adding the average performance to the contribution of each parameter using the following equations (Phadke 1989).

$$ {{Y}}_{\text{opt}} = {{m}} + \left({{{m}}_{\text{Aopt}} {-}{{m}}} \right) + \left({{{m}}_{\text{Bopt}} {-}{{m}}} \right) + \left({{{m}}_{\text{Copt}} {-}{{m}}} \right) $$
(3)
$$ {{m}} = {{T}}/{{n}} $$

where m is the average performance, T the grand total of average readability performance for each combination, n the total number of combinations and m Aopt the average readability for parameter A at its optimum level, m Bopt the average readability at its optimum level, m Copt the average readability for parameter C at its optimum level. For conforming the result predicted, as a rule, the optimum condition will not be one of the experiments already conducted at the optimum condition. So it is a good idea to plan on running an additional few samples at the optimum condition. These confirmation tests serve two purposes. Firstly, they establish the new performance at new (optimum) condition, which can establish the improvement achieved. Secondly, they allow the experimenter to determine how close the estimate is to the results observed (Roy 2001). Expression for calculating the confidence interval is given as:

$$ {\text{CI}} = \sqrt {\frac{F(n1,n2)xVe}{Ne}} $$
(4)

where F (n1, n2) = Tabulated value of F-ratio, Ve = Error variance, Ne = Number of replications.

For the present study, the CI was calculated considering 95 % level of confidence and repeating the experiment for 15° viewing angle four times at optimum condition. As results obtained were quite consistent, therefore experiment was not repeated for more number of iterations. Expected result at optimum condition for 150 viewing angle was calculated and found to be in between 48.77 dB (46.55 + 2.22) and 44.33 dB (46.55–2.22).Since all the values for 150 viewing angle (Table 5) were within the upper (48.77 dB) and lower (44.33 dB) limit set by calculation, therefore it was concluded that for 95 % of the confidence level experimental results are correct. Similarly for 40° viewing angle, the calculation was made and it was again found that for a confidence level of 95 % results were within upper and lower control limit set by calculation.

Once the optimal combination and levels of operational parameters for maximum readability performance in terms of number of words read per minute have been obtained, the final step was to verify the estimated results with the experimental values. Estimated readability performance at optimum condition was calculated by adding the average performance to the contribution of each parameter. Tables 9, 10 show the results of the confirmation experiment using the optimal operating parameters. It can be seen from these tables that the predicted readability performance for the condition A3B3C2 for experiment-1 and A3B3C3 for experiment-2 is very close to the experimental value and the increase of the S/N ratio from the initial operating parameters to the optimal operating parameter is 1.58 dB for experiment-1 and 1.26 dB for experiment-2. Thus, the method discussed in paper increases the readability performance when the reading task is performed at optimum combination of operating parameters and their levels i.e. A3B3C2 for experiment-1 and A3B3C3 for experiment-2. Based on the result of the confirmation test, the readability is improved from 190 to 225 WPM, in experiment-1; 190–215 WPM, in experiment-2. Figure 6 shows the comparative improvement in readability performance for 15° and 40° viewing angle experiments.

Table 9 Results of confirmation test (15° viewing angle)
Table 10 Results of confirmation test (40° viewing angle)
Fig. 6
figure 6

Comparison of performance improvement (readability task) For 15° and 40° viewing angles

Conclusions

The individual influence of the text/background color, viewing distance and character size on the readability performance of the operators of computers have been optimized using Taguchi method. The optimum operating parameters and their levels were found to be A3B3C2 i.e. the black-on-white, viewing distance 60 cm, character size 1.5 deg, for experiment-1 in which viewing angle was maintained at 15°. Similarly, optimum operating parameters were found to be A3B3C3, i.e., the black-on-white, viewing distance = 60 cm and character size 4.00° for experiment-2 in which viewing angle was maintained at 40°. In both the studies, text/background color at level 3(black-on-white) gave the optimum performance (maximum number of WPM). This result is consistent with the finding of Sanders and McCormick (1993); Wang et al. (2002); Wang and Chen (2003). Similarly, in both the experiments it was found that preferable viewing distance is 60 cm. This finding is in agreement with Jaschinski-Krutz (1991); Szeto and Lee (2002); Jaschinski-Kruza (1998). Viewing distance may reflect the appropriateness of various aspects of VDT design. The character size varied from 0.2° to 4.00° for obtaining the maximum readability performance in the two experiments conducted. This is in agreement with results obtained by Knoblauch et al. (1991); Ojanpaa and Nasanen (2003). The finding of this work in terms of preferred viewing angle adopted by the subjects to give better readability performance is 15°. Various viewing angles were suggested but it was found that subjects preferred 15° viewing angle (Turville et al. 1998). In literature, performance measure is found by combining two factors i.e., luminance contrast with character size or viewing distance with character size but combining three factors together i.e., luminance contrast, viewing distance and character size together did not apparently studied.

Thus, it is clear that if the above mentioned factors are combined together at the appropriate level, the readability task performance of the operators will improve and the reading time may be reduced.