Skip to main content
Log in

Meditation, Absorption, Transcendent Experience, and Affect: Tying It All Together Via the Consciousness State Space (CSS) Model

  • ORIGINAL PAPER
  • Published:
Mindfulness Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this paper, we relate meditation practice to participant trait absorption, affect, and transcendent experience. The motivation for this analysis stems from a new neurophenomenological model of consciousness which we recently published, named the consciousness state space. Here, we compare two distinct forms of meditation: mindfulness and transcendental meditation, as well as two different levels of expertise within the mindfulness group. Participants comprised 64 mindfulness practitioners, 18 transcendental meditation practitioners, and 59 healthy controls, who had no prior meditation experience. We further split our mindfulness participants into two groups, using a cutoff point of 2000 h of accumulated experience. We tested three predictions. The first prediction was that contemplative practices should result in increased first-order awareness as a trait, or trait absorption. Our findings revealed that meditators (in general) scored higher on trait absorption than comparable controls, with no difference between meditators from the two traditions. A second prediction was that contemplative practices should result in a reduced sense of regular (narrative) self; hence, transcendent experience should be enhanced for long-term practitioners of meditation. Our findings show that meditators (in general) score higher on the Mystical Scale than comparable controls, with no difference between meditators from the two traditions. We further predicted that contemplative practices should result in lower positive and negative valence, measured by Positive Affect and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) scores on both positive and negative affect scales, as a trait, but this hypothesis was only partly supported by the data.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alexander, C. N., Boyer, R. W., & Alexander, V. K. (1987). Higher states of consciousness in the Vedic Psychology of Maharishi Mahesh Yogi: a theoretical introduction and research review. Modern Science and Vedic Science, 1, 89–126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, C. N., & Langer, E. J. (1990). Higher stages of human development: Perspectives on adult growth. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  • Alexander, C. N., Rainforth, M. V., & Gelderloos, P. (1991). Transcendental meditation, self-actualization, and psychological health: a conceptual overview and statistical meta-analysis. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 6(5), 189–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, D. (1983). Meditation as a treatment for primary dysmenorrhea among women with high and low absorption scores. Doctoral Dissertation, Texas Tech University, Lubbock, TX.

  • Austin, J. H. (1998). Zen and the brain. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berkovich-Ohana, A., & Glicksohn, J. (2014). The consciousness state space—a unifying model for consciousness and self. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–19. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berkovich-Ohana, A., Glicksohn, J., & Goldstein, A. (2011). Temporal cognition changes following mindfulness, but not transcendental meditation practice. In D. Algom, D. Zakay, E. Chajut, S. Shaki, Y. Mama, & V. Shakuf (Eds.), Fechner Day 2011: Proceedings of the 27th Annual Meeting of the International Society for Psychophysics (pp. 245–250). Raanana, Israel.

  • Berkovich-Ohana, A., Glicksohn, J., & Goldstein, A. (2012). Mindfulness-induced changes in gamma band activity—implications for the default mode network, self-reference and attention. Clinical Neurophysiology, 123(4), 700–710.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berkovich-Ohana, A., Dor-Ziderman, Y., Glicksohn, J., & Goldstein, A. (2013a). Alterations in the sense of time, space and body in the mindfulness-trained brain: a neurophenomenologically-guided MEG study. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 912. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00912.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Berkovich-Ohana, A., Glicksohn, J., & Goldstein, A. (2013b). Studying the default mode and its mindfulness-induced changes using EEG functional connectivity. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5, 1–9. doi:10.1093/scan/nst153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowden, D., Gaudry, C., An, S. C., & Gruzelier, J. (2011). A comparative randomised controlled trial of the effects of brain wave vibration training, iyengar yoga, and mindfulness on mood, well-being, and salivary cortisol. Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine, 2012, 1–13. doi:10.1155/2012/234713.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bresnick, T., & Levin, R. (2006). Phenomenal qualities of Ayahuasca ingestion and its relation to fringe consciousness and personality. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 13(9), 5–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, Z., Hood, R. W., Jr., Yang, L., & Watson, P. J. (2011). Mystical experience among Tibetan Buddhists: the common core thesis revisited. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 50(2), 328–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1988). The flow experience and its significance for human psychology. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. S. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience: Psychological studies of flow in consciousness (pp. 15–35). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R. (1999). The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. New York: Harcourt Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. (2012). Self comes to mind: Constructing the conscious brain. New York: Pantheon Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dambrun, M., & Ricard, M. (2011). Self-centeredness and selflessness: a theory of self-based psychological functioning and its consequences for happiness. Review of General Psychology, 15, 138–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R. J., & Goleman, D. J. (1977). The role of attention in meditation and hypnosis: a psychobiological perspective on transformations of consciousness. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 25(4), 291–308. doi:10.1080/00207147708415986.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R. J., Goleman, D. J., & Schwartz, G. E. (1976). Attentional and affective concomitants of meditation: a cross-sectional study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 85(2), 235–238.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R., Kabat-Zinn, J., Schumacher, J., Rosenkranz, M., Muller, D., Santorelli, S., et al. (2003). Alterations in brain and immune function produced by mindfulness meditation. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65(4), 564–570. doi:10.1097/01.PSY.0000077505.67574.E3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deikman, A. J. (2000). A functional approach to mysticism. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 7(11–12), 75–92.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ding, X., Tang, Y.-Y., Tang, R., & Posner, M. I. (2014). Improving creativity performance by short-term meditation. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 10(1), 1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dor-Ziderman, Y., Berkovich-Ohana, A., Glicksohn, J., & Goldstein, A. (2013). Mindfulness-induced selflessness: a MEG neurophenomenological study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 582. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00582.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dreyfus, G., & Thompson, E. (2007). Asian perspectives: Indian theories of mind. In E. Thompson, P. D. Zelazo, & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of consciousness (pp. 89–114). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Easterlin, B. L., & Cardeña, E. (1998–99). Cognitive and emotional differences between short- and long-term Vipassana meditators. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 18(1), 69–81.

  • Gallagher, S. (2000). Philosophical conceptions of the self: implications for cognitive science. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(1), 14–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S., & Zahavi, D. (2008). The phenomenological mind: An introduction to philosophy of mind and cognitive science. New York: Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glicksohn, J. (1998). States of consciousness and symbolic cognition. The Journal of Mind and Behavior, 19(2), 105–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glicksohn, J., & Barrett, T. R. (2003). Absorption and hallucinatory experience. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17(7), 833–849.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glicksohn, J., & Berkovich-Ohana, A. (2012). Absorption, immersion, and consciousness. In J. Gackenbach (Ed.), Video game play and consciousness (pp. 83–99). New York: Nova.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glicksohn, J., Gvirtsman, D., & Offer, S. (1995–96). The compensatory nature of mood: a single-subject time-series approach. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 15(4), 385–396.

  • Goleman, D. (1988). The meditative mind—the varieties of meditative experience. New York: GP Putnam & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gombrich, R. F. (1995). Theravada Buddhism. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grant, J. A., Duerden, E. G., Courtemanche, J., Cherkasova, M., Duncan, G. H., & Rainville, P. (2013). Cortical thickness, mental absorption and meditative practice: possible implications for disorders of attention. Biological Psychology, 92(2), 275–281.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gunaratana, B., & Gunaratana, H. (2011). Mindfulness in plain English. USA: Wisdom Publications Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartman, D., & Zimberoff, D. (2008). Higher stages of human development. Journal of Heart-Centered Therapies, 11, 3–95

  • Hölzel, B., & Ott, U. (2006). Relationships between meditation depth, absorption, meditation practice, and mindfulness: a latent variable approach. Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 38(2), 179–199.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hölzel, B. K., Lazar, S. W., Gard, T., Schuman-Olivier, Z., Vago, D. R., & Ott, U. (2011). How does mindfulness meditation work? Proposing mechanisms of action from a conceptual and neural perspective. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(6), 537–559.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hood, R. W., Jr. (1975). The construction and preliminary validation of a measure of reported mystical experience. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 14, 29–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, H. T. (2000). Experiences of radical personal transformation in mysticism, religious conversion, and psychosis: a review of the varieties, processes, and consequences of the numinous. The Journal of Mind and Behavior, 21(4), 353–397.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, H. T. (2007). “Dark nights of the soul”: phenomenology and neurocognition of spiritual suffering in mysticism and psychosis. Review of General Psychology, 11(3), 209–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ivanovski, B., & Malhi, G. S. (2007). The psychological and neurophysiological concomitants of mindfulness forms of meditation. Acta Neuropsychiatrica, 19(2), 76–91. doi:10.1111/j.1601-5215.2007.00175.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1958). The varieties of religious experience. New York: The Modern Library.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lazar, A., & Kravetz, S. (2005). Responses to the Mystical Scale by religious Jewish persons: a comparison of structural models of mystical experience. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 15(1), 51–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levin, J., & Steele, L. (2005). The transcendent experience: conceptual, theoretical, and epidemiologic perspectives. Explore: The Journal of Science and Healing, 1(2), 89–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. J. (2008). Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(4), 163–169.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mahesh Yogi, M. (1966). The science of being and the art of living. London: International SRM.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maslow, A. H. (1971). The farther reaches of human nature. New York: Viking.

    Google Scholar 

  • May, C. J., Weyker, J. R., Spengel, S. K., Finkler, L. J., & Hendrix, S. E. (2012). Tracking longitudinal changes in affect and mindfulness caused by concentration and loving-kindness meditation with hierarchical linear modeling. Mindfulness, 3(4), 1–10. doi:10.1007/s12671-012-0172-8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morin, A. (2006). Levels of consciousness and self-awareness: a comparison and integration of various neurocognitive views. Consciousness and Cognition, 15(2), 358–371.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, P. L. (1990). The technology of the praeternatural: an empirically based model of transpersonal experiences. The Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 22(1), 35–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ott, U. (2007). States of absorption: In search of neurobiological foundations. In G. A. Jamieson (Ed.), Hypnosis and conscious states: The cognitive neuroscience perspective (pp. 257–270). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, J. (2000). Mental attention, consciousness, and the progressive emergence of wisdom. Journal of Adult Development, 7, 241–254.

  • Pagano, R. R., & Warrenburg, S. (1983). Meditation: In search of a unique effect. In R. J. Davidson, G. Schwartz, & D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and self regulation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 3, pp. 152–210). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. A., & Carroll, J. M. (1999). On the bipolarity of positive and negative affect. Psychological Bulletin, 125(1), 3–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sears, S., & Kraus, S. (2009). I think therefore I om: cognitive distortions and coping style as mediators for the effects of mindfulness meditation on anxiety, positive and negative affect, and hope. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 65(6), 561–573.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stace, W. T. (1961). Mysticism and philosophy. Los Angeles: Jeremy P. Tarcher.

    Google Scholar 

  • Studerus, E., Gamma, A., Kometer, M., & Vollenweider, F. X. (2012). Prediction of psilocybin response in healthy volunteers. PLoS ONE, 7(2), e30800.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tang, Y.-Y., Ma, Y., Wang, J., Fan, Y., Feng, S., Lu, Q., Yu, Q., Sui, D., Rothbart, M. K., Fan, M., & Posner, M. I. (2007). Short-term meditation training improves attention and self-regulation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(43), 17152–17156. doi:10.1073/pnas.0707678104.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tang, Y.-Y., Hölzel, B. K., & Posner, M. I. (2015). The neuroscience of mindfulness meditation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(4), 213–225.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tellegen, A. (1982). Multidimensional personality questionnaire. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota.

    Google Scholar 

  • Travis, F., & Shear, J. (2010). Focused attention, open monitoring and automatic self-transcending: categories to organize meditations from Vedic, Buddhist and Chinese traditions. Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 1110–1118.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Travis, F., Tecce, J., Arenander, A., & Wallace, R. K. (2002). Patterns of EEG coherence, power, and contingent negative variation characterize the integration of transcendental and waking states. Biological Psychology, 61(3), 293–319.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vago, D. R., & Silbersweig, D. A. (2012). Self-awareness, self-regulation, and self-transcendence (S-ART): a framework for understanding the neurobiological mechanisms of mindfulness. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 296. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00296.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Valentine, E. R., & Sweet, P. L. G. (1999). Meditation and attention: a comparison of the effects of concentrative and mindfulness meditation on sustained attention. Mental Health, Religion and Culture, 2(1), 59–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wachholtz, A. B. (2006). Does spirituality matter? Effects of meditative content and orientation on migraineurs. Bowling Green State University.

  • Wachholtz, A. B., & Pargament, K. I. (2005). Is spirituality a critical ingredient of meditation? Comparing the effects of spiritual meditation, secular meditation, and relaxation on spiritual, psychological, cardiac, and pain outcomes. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 28(4), 369–384.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Walsh, R., & Shapiro, S. L. (2006). The meeting of meditative disciplines and western psychology: a mutually enriching dialogue. American Psychologist, 61(3), 227–239.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the Mind and Life Institute, Francisco J. Varela Research Award 2012-Varela-Berkovich, and by a grant from the Bial Foundation (27/10).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Aviva Berkovich-Ohana.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Berkovich-Ohana, A., Glicksohn, J. Meditation, Absorption, Transcendent Experience, and Affect: Tying It All Together Via the Consciousness State Space (CSS) Model. Mindfulness 8, 68–77 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-015-0481-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-015-0481-9

Keywords

Navigation