Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Towards Diffractive Transdisciplinarity: Integrating Gender Knowledge into the Practice of Neuroscientific Research

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Neuroethics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The current neurosciences contribute to the construction of gender/sex to a high degree. Moreover, the subject of gender/sex differences in cognitive abilities attracts an immense public interest. At the same time, the entanglement of gender and science has been shown in many theoretical and empirical analyses. Although the body of literature is very extensive and differentiated with regards to the dimensions of ‘neuroscience of gender’ and ‘gender in neuroscience’, the feeding back of these findings into the field of neuroscience remains a desideratum. Especially, the question of how gender knowledge, i.e. insights from feminist theory on gender/sex and from gender and science studies on knowledge production, may be integrated and applied within the neurosciences has been strongly neglected. Presumably due to their epistemic culture and epistemological presuppositions, these critical engagements are conceived as externalist by critical scholars and neuroscientists alike. In this context, the question arises of how substantiated gender knowledge may be accounted for in neuroscientific research practice? The article outlines methodological considerations for a critical research agenda in the cognitive neurosciences. I present thoughts on how insights and expertise from gender and science studies can be taken into account in the neuroscientific practice of knowledge production. Starting from the assumption that changes in neuroscientific research practices are possible, my aim is to point out possibilities of integrating gender knowledge into the neurosciences.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. I use this unfamiliar combination of the two terms ‘sex’ and ‘gender’ to mark the complexity of these concepts and their entanglements. This will be clarified later in this article.

  2. Here I refer to Evelyn Fox Keller’s dimensions of feminist analysis of the gender-science-system [10]: women in science, the analysis of structural and symbolic explanations for the underrepresentation of women in the field of science; b) science of gender, the analysis of the scientific construction of gender/sex; and c) gender in science, the analysis of processes of gendering in research aims, theory development, methods and interpretations.

  3. Some refer to this field as ‘gender and science studies’ or ‘gender studies of science’.

  4. The works by Deboleena Roy and by Anelis Kaiser and their colleagues can be regarded as the rare exceptions to this rule [cf. 2, 2831].

  5. Gender studies as a transdisciplinary field are viewed as being situated mainly in the humanities and social sciences.

  6. Isabelle Dussauge & Anelis Kaiser: call for papers for the workshop “NeuroGenderings: Critical Studies of the Sexed Brain”, Uppsala University, March 25–27, 2010.

  7. Here I refer to unpublished data from an analysis of neuroscientific research articles on mental rotation using functional MRI as an experimental paradigm. However, a scan of research papers on other neuroscientific topics and approaches suggests that these observations can be generalised.

  8. This strand of scholarship has been described as “material feminisms” [44] or “new feminist materialism” [45].

  9. I elaborate on this point in more detail elsewhere [21].

  10. In this paper I cannot follow up the criticism that exists against ‘innovation’ and the scientific idea of linear progress as well as the problem of economic appropriation of critical thinking.

  11. In order to think about the third aspect there needs to be some awareness, of course, that there actually might be a ‘problem with gender’.

References

  1. Cahill, L. 2006. Why sex matters for neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 7(6): 484.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Kaiser, A., S. Haller, S. Schmitz, and C. Nitsch. 2009. On sex/gender related similarities and differences in fMRI language research. Brain Research Reviews 61: 49–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Sommer, I.E.C., A. Aleman, A. Bouma, and R.S. Kahn. 2004. Do women really have more bilateral language representation than men? A meta-analysis of functional imaging studies. Brain 127(8): 1845–1852.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Wallentin, M. 2009. Putative sex differences in verbal abilities and language cortex: A critical review. Brain and Language 108(3): 175–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Jordan, K. 2010. The brain between sex and gender: Women and men from a neuroscientific perspective. In Gender and sex in biomedicine: Theories, methodologies, results, ed. I. Klinge and C. Wiesemann, 79–99. Göttingen: Universitätsverlag Göttingen.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Fine, C. 2008. Will working mothers’ brains explode? The popular new genre of neurosexism. Neuroethics 1: 69–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Fine, C. 2010. Delusions of gender: How our minds, society, and neurosexism create difference. New York: W. W. Norton & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  8. DesAutels, P. 2010. Sex differences and neuroethics. Philosophical Psychology 23(1): 95–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Hüsing, B. 2008. Technology assessment of neuroimaging: Sex and gender perspectives. In Sexualized brains: Scientific modeling of emotional intelligence from a cultural perspective, ed. N.C. Karafyllis and G. Ulshöfer, 103–116. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Keller, E.F. 1995. Origin, history, and politics of the subject called ‘gender and science’—A first person account. In Handbook of science and technology studies, ed. S. Jasanoff, G.E. Markle, J.C. Petersen, and T. Pinch, 80–95. Thousand Oaks: Sage.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  11. Jordan-Young, R.M. 2010. Brain storm: The flaws in the science of sex differences. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Star, S.L. 1979. Sex differences and the dichotomization of the brain: Methods, limits, and problems in research on consciousness. In Genes and gender: Pitfalls in research on sex and gender, ed. R. Hubbard and M. Lowe, 113–130. New York: Gordian Press.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Bleier, R. 1991. Gender ideology and the brain: Sex differences research. In Women and men: New perspectives on gender differences, ed. M.T. Notman and C.C. Nadelson, 63–73. Washington: American Psychiatric Press.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Fausto-Sterling, A. 2000. Sexing the body: Gender politics and the construction of sexuality. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Schmitz, S. 2002. Hirnforschung und Geschlecht: Eine kritische Analyse im Rahmen der Genderforschung in den Naturwissenschaften. In Gender Studies: Denkachsen und Perspektiven der Geschlechterforschung, ed. I. Bauer and J. Neissl, 109–126. Innsbruck: StudienVerlag.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Schmitz, S. 2010. Sex, gender, and the brain—Biological determinism versus socio-cultural constructivism. In Gender and sex in biomedicine: Theories, methodologies, results, ed. I. Klinge and C. Wiesemann, 57–76. Göttingen: Universitätsverlag Göttingen.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Hegarty, P. 1997. Materializing the hypothalamus: A performative account of the ‘gay brain’. Feminism & Psychology 7(3): 355–372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Karafyllis, N.C., and G. Ulshöfer (ed.). 2008. Sexualized brains: Scientific modeling of emotional intelligence from a cultural perspective. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Nikoleyczik, K. 2004. NormKörper: ‘Geschlecht’ und ‘Rasse’ in biomedizinischen Bildern. In Grenzgänge: Genderforschung in Informatik und Naturwissenschaften, ed. S. Schmitz and B. Schinzel, 133–148. Königstein: Ulrike Helmer Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Schinzel, B. 2006. The body in medical imaging between reality and construction. Poiesis & Praxis 4(3): 185–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Nikoleyczik, K. 2010. Zur Re-Produktion von Differenz in der neurowissenschaftlichen Bildgebung. In Epistemologie und Differenz: Zur Reproduktion in den Wissenschaften, ed. B.Bock von Wülfingen and U. Frietsch, 171–186. Bielefeld: transcript.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Wilson, E.A. 1998. Neural geographies: Feminism and the microstructure of cognition. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Wilson, E.A. 2004. Psychosomatic: Feminism and the neurological body. Durham: Duke University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Fullagar, S. 2009. Negotiating the neurochemical self: Anti-depressant consumption in women’s recovery from depression. Health 13(4): 389–406.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Beaulieu, A. 2000. The brain at the end of the rainbow: The promises of brain scans in the research field and in the media. In Wild science: Reading feminism, medicine, and the media, ed. J. Marchessault and K. Sawchuk, 39–52. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Palm, K. 2004. Gender—eine unbekannte Kategorie in den Naturwissenschaften? In Gender Studies: Wissenschaftstheorien und Gesellschaftskritik, ed. T. Frey Steffen, C. Rosenthal, and A. Väth, 97–109. Würzburg: Königshausen & Neumann.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Palm, K. 2004. Was bringt die Genderforschung eigentlich den Naturwissenschaften? In Grenzgänge: Genderforschung in Informatik und Naturwissenschaften, ed. S. Schmitz and B. Schinzel, 50–64. Königstein: Ulrike Helmer Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Kaiser, A., E. Kuenzli, D. Zappatore, and C. Nitsch. 2007. On females’ lateral and males’ bilateral activation during language production: A fMRI study. International Journal of Psychophysiology 63: 192–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Roy, D., N.L. Angelini, H. Fujieda, G.M. Brown, and D.D. Belsham. 2001. Cyclical regulation of GnRH gene expression in GT1-7 GnRH-secreting neurons by melatonin. Endocrinology 142: 4711–4720.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Roy, D., and D.D. Belsham. 2002. Melatonin receptor activation regulates gonadotropin-releasing hormone (GnRH) gene expression and secretion in GT1-7 GnRH neurons: Signal transduction mechanisms. Journal of Biological Chemistry 277: 251–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Roy, D. 2004. Feminist theory in science: Working toward a practical transformation. Hypatia 19(1): 255–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Knorr-Cetina, K.D. 1999. Epistemic cultures: How the sciences make knowledge. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Haig, D. 2004. The inexorable rise of gender and the decline of sex: Social change in academic titles, 1945–2001. Archives of Sexual Behavior 33: 87–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Krieger, N. 2003. Genders, sexes, and health: What are the connections—and why does it matter? International Journal of Epidemiology 32(4): 652–657.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Germon, J. 2009. Gender—A genealogy of an idea. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Ceci, S., and W. Williams (ed.). 2000. Nature/nurture debate: The essential readings. Sussex: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Fausto-Sterling, A. 2003. The problem with sex/gender and nature/nurture. In Debating biology, ed. S.J. Williams, L. Birke, and G.A. Bendelow, 123–132. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  38. Lippa, R.A. 2005. Gender, nature, and nurture. London: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  39. West, C., and D.H. Zimmerman. 1987. Doing gender. Gender and Society 1(2): 125–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Butler, J. 1990. Gender trouble: Feminism and the subversion of identity. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Butler, J. 1993. Bodies that matter: On the discursive limits of “sex”. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Barad, K. 2007. Meeting the universe halfway: Quantum physics and the entanglement of matter and meaning. Durham: Duke University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Barad, K. 2003. Posthumanist performativity: Toward an understanding of how matter comes to matter. Signs: Journal of Women in Culture and Society 28(3): 801–831.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Alaimo, S., and S. Hekman. 2008. Material feminisms. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  45. van der Tuin, I. 2009. On second- and third-wave feminist epistemology. Australian Feminist Studies 24(59): 17–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Lettow, S. 2010. Bio-technosciences in philosophy: Challenges and perspectives for gender studies in philosophy. Diogenes 225: 127–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Haraway, D.J. 1997. Modest_witness@second_millennium. FemaleMan©_meets_oncomouse™: Feminism and technoscience. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Rheinberger, H.-J. 1997. Toward a history of epistemic things: Synthesizing proteins in the test tube. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  49. Deuber-Mankowsky, A. 2008. Eine Frage des Wissens: Gender als epistemisches Ding. In Gender Goes Life: Die Lebenswissenschaften als Herausforderung für die Gender Studies, ed. M.L. Angerer and C. König, 137–162. Bielefeld: transcript.

    Google Scholar 

  50. Huber, C. 2009. Interdependence of theoretical concepts and neuroimaging data. Poiesis & Praxis 6(3–4): 203–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Snow, C.P. 1993. The two cultures. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  52. Roepstorff, A. 2002. Transforming subjects into objectivity—An “ethnography of knowledge” in a brain imaging laboratory. FOLK—Journal of the Danish Ethnographic Society 44: 145–170.

    Google Scholar 

  53. Jack, A.I., and A. Roepstorff. 2002. Introspection and cognitive brain mapping: from stimulus-response to script-report. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6(8): 333–339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Clénin, D., and B. Pieper. 2009. Aufschreiben, was sowieso schon da ist: Von der Sprachlosigkeit zum Sprachvermögen. feldenkrais zeit—Journal für somatisches Lernen 10: 15–19.

    Google Scholar 

  55. Pieper, B., and D. Clénin. 2010. Verkörperte Selbst- und Fremdwahrnehmung sozialen Handelns: Eine leiborientierte praktisch-theoretische Forschungsperspektive. In Die Körperlichkeit sozialen Handelns: Soziale Ordnung jenseits von Normen und Institutionen, ed. F. Böhle and M. Weihrich, 261–298. Bielefeld: transcript.

    Google Scholar 

  56. Greber, E. 2005. Theoretische Grundüberlegungen zur Wissenschaftsgeschichtsschreibung und –forschung unter der Perspektive der Geschlechterdifferenz. In Gendered Academia: Wissenschaft und Geschlechterdifferenz 1890–1945, ed. M. Kauko, S. Mieszkowski, and A. Tischel, 11–40. Göttingen: Wallstein.

    Google Scholar 

  57. Parvizi, J. 2009. Corticocentric myopia: Old bias in new cognitive sciences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13(8): 354–359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Schiebinger, L. 2008. Gendered innovations in science and engineering. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  59. Feldenkrais, M. 1972. Awareness through movement. San Francisco: Harper Collins.

    Google Scholar 

  60. Kueffer, C., G. Hadorn, G. Bammer, L. Kerkhoff, and C. Pohl. 2007. Towards a publication culture in transdisciplinary research. GAIA - Ecological Perspectives for Science and Society 16(1): 22–26.

    Google Scholar 

  61. Loibl, M.C. 2005. Spannungen in Forschungsteams: Hintergründe und Methoden zum konstruktiven Abbau von Konflikten in inter- und transdisziplinären Projekten. Heidelberg: Verlag für Systemische Forschung im Carl-Auer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  62. Weingart, P., and N. Stehr. 2000. Practising interdisciplinarity. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.

    Google Scholar 

  63. Thompson-Klein, J., W. Grossenbacher-Mansuy, R. Häberli, A. Bill, R.W. Scholz, and M. Welti (ed.). 2001. Transdisciplinarity—joint problem solving among science, technology, and society: An effective way for managing complexity. Basel: Birkhäuser.

    Google Scholar 

  64. Bergmann, M. 2003. Indikatoren für eine diskursive Evaluation transdisziplinärer Forschung. Technikfolgenabschätzung-Theorie und Praxis 12(1): 65–75.

    Google Scholar 

  65. Defila, R., A. Di Giulio, and M. Scheuermann. 2006. Forschungsverbundmanagement: Handbuch für die Gestaltung inter- und transdisziplinärer Projekte. Zürich: vdf Hochschulverlag.

    Google Scholar 

  66. Monteiro, M., and E. Keating. 2009. Managing misunderstandings: The role of language in interdisciplinary scientific collaboration. Science Communication 31(1): 6–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Shulman, B. 1996. What if we changed our axioms? A feminist inquiry into the foundations of mathematics. Configurations 4(3): 427–451.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Kerr, E.A. 1998. Toward a feminist natural science: Linking theory and practice. Women’s Studies International Forum 21(1): 95–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Barad, K. 2000. Reconceiving scientific literacy as agential literacy, or learning how to intra-act responsibly within the world. In Doing culture + science, ed. R. Reid and S. Traweek, 221–258. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  70. Mayberry, M., B. Subramaniam, and L.H. Weasel (eds.). 2001. Feminist science studies: A new generation. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  71. Klinge, I., and M. Bosch. 2005. Transforming research methodologies in EU life sciences and biomedicine. European Journal of Women’s Studies 12(3): 377–395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Roy, D. 2008. Should feminists clone? And if so, how? Notes from an implicated modest witness. Australian Feminist Studies 23(56): 225–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Gilbert, S.F., and A. Fausto-Sterling. 2003. Educating for social responsibility: Changing the syllabus of developmental biology. International Journal of Developmental Biology 47(2–3): 237–244.

    Google Scholar 

  74. Schmitz, S., and K. Nikoleyczik. 2009. Interdisciplinary and gender-sensitive teaching: Didactical concepts and technical support. International Journal of Innovation in Education 1(1): 81–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Steinbach, J., and B. Jansen-Schulz (ed.). 2009. Gender im Experiment-Gender in Experience: Ein Best-Practice-Handbuch zur Integration von Genderaspekten in naturwissenschaftliche und technische Lehre. Berlin: Universitätsverlag TU Berlin.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Some ideas set out in this paper have been previously presented by the author at the workshop “NeuroGenderings: Critical Studies of the Sexed Brain” held at Uppsala University, March 25–27, 2010. I wish to thank the workshop participants as well as my students for useful remarks and discussions. Further, I gratefully acknowledge insightful comments on earlier versions of the manuscript by Nicole Scheifhacken and the anonymous reviewers, which helped to improve the article. Finally, I thank Anita Hopes for stylistic support and proofreading.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Katrin Nikoleyczik.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Nikoleyczik, K. Towards Diffractive Transdisciplinarity: Integrating Gender Knowledge into the Practice of Neuroscientific Research. Neuroethics 5, 231–245 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12152-011-9135-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12152-011-9135-3

Keywords

Navigation