Skip to main content
Log in

Primate Sociality to Human Cooperation

Why Us and Not Them?

  • Published:
Human Nature Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Developmental psychologists identify propensities for social engagement in human infants that are less evident in other apes; Sarah Hrdy links these social propensities to novel features of human childrearing. Unlike other ape mothers, humans can bear a new baby before the previous child is independent because they have help. This help alters maternal trade-offs and so imposes new selection pressures on infants and young children to actively engage their caretakers’ attention and commitment. Such distinctive childrearing is part of our grandmothering life history. While consequences for other cooperative activities must surely follow, the novel rearing environments set up by helpful grandmothering can explain why natural selection escalated preferences and motivations for interactivity in our lineage in the first place, and why, unlike other aspects of infant development, social sensitivities are not delayed in humans compared with genus Pan.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alvarez, H. P. (2004). Residence groups among hunter gatherers: a review of the claims and evidence for patrilocal bands. In B. Chapais & C. M. Berman (Eds.), Kinship and behavior in primates (pp. 420–442). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aureli, F., & de Waal, F. B. M. (Eds.). (2000). Natural conflict resolution. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baillargeon, R., Scott, R. M., & He, Z. (2010). False-belief understanding in infants. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(3), 110–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bard, K. A. (1996). Responsive care: Behavioral intervention for nursery-reared chimpanzees. In Landau, V. I. (ed.) The 1996 ChimpanZoo Conference. ChimpanZoo, Sponsored Program of the Jane Goodall Institute, Tucson.

  • Bard, K. A. (2012). Emotional engagement: How chimpanzee minds develop. In F. B. M. de Waal & P. F. Ferrari (Eds.), The primate mind (pp. 224–245). Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bard, K. A., & Gardener, K. H. (1996). Influences on development in infant chimpanzees: Enculturation, temperament, and cognition. In A. E. Russon, K. A. Bard, & S. T. Parker (Eds.), Reaching into thought: The minds of the great apes (pp. 235–256). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bard, K. A., Todd, B. K., Bernier, C., Love, J., & Leavens, D. A. (2006). Self-awareness in human and chimpanzee infants: what is measured and what is meant by the Mark and Mirror Test? Infancy, 9(2), 191–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bard, K. A., Brent, L., Lester, B., Worobey, J., & Suomi, S. J. (2011). Neurobehavioural integrity of chimpanzee newborns: comparisons across groups and across species reveal gene-environment interaction effects. Infant and Child Development, 20, 47–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bayley, N. (1969). Bayley scales of infant development. New York: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boehm, C. (1999). Hierarchy in the forest: The evolution of egalitarian behavior. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boesch, C. (2012). The ecology and evolution of social behavior and cognition in primates. In J. Vonk & T. Shackelford (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of comparative evolutionary psychology (pp. 486–503). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowles, S., & Gintis, H. (2011). A cooperative species: Human reciprocity and its evolution. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Brazelton, T. B. (1984). Neonatal behavioral assessment scale (2nd ed.). Clinics in Developmental Medicine (Vol. 88). Philadelphia: Lippincott.

  • Bribiescas, R. G. (2006). Men: Evolutionary and life history. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burton-Chellew, M. N., & West, S. A. (2013). Prosocial preferences do not explain human cooperation in public-goods games. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 110(1), 216–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Call, J. (2009). Contrasting the social cognition of humans and nonhuman apes: the shared intentionality hypothesis. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1, 368–379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Science, 12, 187–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cartmill, M. (2010). The human (r)evolution(s). Evolutionary Anthropology, 19(3), 89–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charnov, E. L. (1993). Life history invariants: Some explorations of symmetry in evolutionary ecology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Charnov, E., & Berrigan, D. (1993). Why do female primates have such long lifespans and so few babies? Or, life in the slow lane. Evolutionary Anthropology, 1(6), 191–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheney, D. L. (2011). Extent and limits of cooperation in animals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 108(suppl. 2), 10902–10909.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (2007). Baboon metaphysics: The evolution of a social mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Chisholm, J. S. (2003). Uncertainty, contingency and attachment: A life history theory of theory of mind. In K. Sterelny & J. Fitness (Eds.), From mating to mentality: Evaluating evolutionary psychology (pp. 125–153). Hove: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coxworth, J. E. (2013) First among equals: Male-male competition among the Bardi of northwestern Australia and its implications for human evolution. PhD. Dissertation. Anthropology. University of Utah.

  • Darwin, C. (1981). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. Photoreproduction of the 1871 edition published by J. Murray, London. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

  • De Waal, F. B. M. (2000). Primates—a natural heritage of conflict resolution. Science, 289(479), 586–590.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Waal, F. B. M. (2011). What is an animal emotion? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1224, 191–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Waal, F. B. M. (2012). The antiquity of empathy. Science, 336(6083), 874–876.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Waal, F. B. M., & Suchak, M. (2010). Prosocial primates: selfish and unselfish motivations. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B: Biological Sciences, 365, 2711–2722.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Waal, F. B. M., Boesch, C., Horner, V., & Whiten, A. (2008). Comparing social skills of children and apes. Science, 319, 569.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (2010). The social role of touch in humans and primates: behavioural function and neurobiological mechanisms. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, 260–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. I. M., & Shultz, S. (2007). Understanding primate brain evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B: Biological Sciences, 362, 649–658.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, E., & Fischbacker, U. (2003). The nature of human altruism. Nature, 425, 785–791.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, R. A. (1958). The genetical theory of natural selection. New York: Dover.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallup, G. (1970). Chimpanzees: self-recognition. Science, 167, 186–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Godfrey, L. R., & Sutherland, M. R. (1996). Paradox of peramorphic paedomorphosis: heterochrony and human evolution. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 99, 17–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gómez, J.-C. (2004). Apes, monkeys, children, and the growth of mind. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A. (2009). The philosophical baby. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A., Meltzoff, A. N., & Kuhl, P. K. (1999). The scientist in the crib: What early learning tells us about the mind. New York: Harper Collins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gould, S. J. (1977). Ontogeny and phylogeny. Cambridge: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, P. B., & Anderson, K. G. (2010). Fatherhood: Evolution and human paternal behavior. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grice, H. P. (1957). Meaning. The Philosophical Review, 66(3), 377–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J. (2012). The righteous mind: Why good people are divided by politics and religion. New York: Pantheon Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamlin, K. J., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2010). Three-month-olds show a negativity bias in their social evaluations. Developmental Science, 13(6), 923–929.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamlin, K. J., Wynn, K., Bloom, P., & Mahajan, N. (2011). How infants and toddlers react to antisocial others. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 108(50), 19931–19936.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hare, B. (2007). From nonhuman to human mind. What changed and why? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 60–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hare, B. (2011). From hominoid to hominid mind: what changed and why? Annual Review of Anthropology, 40, 293–309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hare, B., & Tan, J. (2012). What cooperative abilities did we inherit as an ape? In F. B. M. de Waal & P. F. Ferrari (Eds.), The primate mind (pp. 175–194). Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hare, B., Wobber, V., & Wrangham, R. (2012). The self-domestication hypothesis: evolution of bonobo psychology is due to selection against aggression. Animal Behaviour, 83(3), 573–585.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K. (1990). Why do men hunt? Some benefits for risky strategies. In E. Cashdan (Ed.), Risk and uncertainty in tribal and peasant economies (pp. 145–166). Boulder: Westview Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K. (1993a). Why hunter-gatherers work: an ancient version of the problem of public goods. Current Anthropology, 34(4), 341–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K. (1993b). On why male foragers hunt and share food: reply to Hill and Kaplan. Current Anthropology, 34(5), 706–710.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K. (2003). Grandmothers and the evolution of human longevity. American Journal Human Biology, 15, 380–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K. (2004). Mating, parenting, and the evolution of human pair bonds. In B. Chapais & C. Berman (Eds.), Kinship and behavior in primates (pp. 443–473). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K. (2006a). Slow life histories and human evolution. In K. Hawkes & R. Paine (Eds.), The evolution of human life history (pp. 95–126). Santa Fe: SAR Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K. (2006b). Life history theory and human evolution. In K. Hawkes & R. Paine (Eds.), The evolution of human life history (pp. 45–93). Santa Fe: SAR Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., & Bliege Bird, R. (2002). Showing off, handicap signaling and the evolution of men’s work. Evolutionary Anthropology, 11, 58–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J. F., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (1989). Hardworking Hadza grandmothers. In V. Standen & R. A. Foley (Eds.), Comparative socioecology: The behavioural ecology of humans and other mammals (pp. 341–366). London: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J. F., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (1991). Hunting income patterns among the Hadza: big game, common goods, foraging goals, and the evolution of the human diet. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B: Biological Sciences, 334, 243–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J. F., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (1997). Hadza women’s time allocation, offspring provisioning and the evolution of post-menopausal lifespans. Current Anthropology, 38(4), 551–577.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J. F., Blurton Jones, N. G., Alvarez, H. P., & Charnov, E. L. (1998). Grandmothering, menopause, and the evolution of human life histories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 95(3), 1336–1339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J. F., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (2001). Hunting and nuclear families: some lessons from the Hadza about men’s work. Current Anthropology, 42(5), 681–709.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J. F., & Coxworth, J. E. (2010). Family provisioning is not the only reason men hunt. Current Anthropology, 52(2), 259–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Science, 33(2–3), 61–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, E., Call, J., Hernandez-Lloreda, M., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: the cultural intelligence hypothesis. Science, 317, 1360–1366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, E., Hare, B., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2010a). Differences in the cognitive skills of Bonobos and Chimpanzees. PLoS ONE, 5(8), e12438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, E., Hernandez-Lloreda, M., Call, J., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2010b). The structure of individual differences in the cognitive abilities of children and chimpanzees. Psychological Science, 21, 102–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, K., & Hurtado, A. M. (2009). Cooperative breeding in South American hunter-gatherers. Proceedings of the Royal Society, Series B: Biological Sciences, 276(1674), 3863–3870.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, K. R., Walker, R. S., Božičević, M., Eder, J., Headland, T., Hewlett, B., et al. (2011). Co-residence patterns in hunter-gatherer societies show unique human social structure. Science, 331, 1286–1289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • House, B. R., Henrich, J., Brosnan, S. F., & Silk, J. B. (2012). The ontogeny of human prosociality: behavioral experiments with children aged 3 to 8. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33(2012), 291–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howell, N. (2010). Life histories of the Dobe !Kung. Berkeley: University of Califronia Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hrdy, S. B. (1999). Mother Nature: Maternal instincts and how they shape the human species. New York: Ballantine Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hrdy, S. B. (2008) Cooperative breeding and the paradox of facultative fatherhood. In The neurobiology of parental care (pp. 405–414). New York: Academic.

  • Hrdy, S. B. (2009). Mothers and others: The evolutionary origins of mutual understanding. Cambridge: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hrdy, S. B. (2013). Development + social selection in the emergence of “emotionally modern” humans. In C. L. Meehan & A. N. Crittenden (Eds.), Origins and implications of the evolution of childhood. Santa Fe: SAR Press (in press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Jaeggi, A. V., Burkart, J. M., & van Schaik, C. P. (2010). On the psychology of cooperation in humans and other primates: combining the natural history and experimental evidence of prosociality. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B: Biological Sciences, 365, 2723–2735.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (2012). Thinking fast and thinking slow. New York: Farrar, Straus, and Giroux.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaminski, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Chimpanzees know what others know, but not what they believe. Cognition, 109, 224–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, H., Hill, K., Lancaster, J., & Hurtado, A. M. (2000). A theory of human life history evolution: diet, intelligence, and longevity. Evolutionary Anthropology, 9, 156–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, H., Gurven, M., Winking, J., Hooper, P., & Stieglitz, J. (2010). Learning, menopause and the human adaptive complex. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1204, 30–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kappeler, P. M., & van Schaik, C. P. (Eds.). (2007). Cooperation in primates and humans: Mechanisms and evolution. Heidelberg: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kappeler, P. M., & Silk, J. B. (Eds.). (2010). Mind the gap: Tracing the origins of human universals. Heidelberg: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kellogg, W. N., & Kellogg, L. A. (1933). The ape and the child: A study of early environmental influence upon early behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, P. K., Coxworth, J. E., & Hawkes, K. (2012). Increased longevity evolves from grandmothering. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B: Biological Sciences, 279, 4880-4884. doi:10.1098/rspb.2012.1751.

  • King, M.-C., & Wilson, A. (1975). Evolution at two levels in humans and chimpanzees. Science, 188, 107–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Konner, M. J. (1972). Aspects of the developmental ethology of a foraging people. In N. Blurton Jones (Ed.), Ethological studies of child behavior (pp. 285–304). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Konner, M. J. (2010). The evolution of childhood: Relationships, emotion, mind. Cambridge: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kramer, K. L. (2011). The evolution of human parental care and recruitment of juvenile help. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 26, 533–540.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kramer, K. L., & Ellison, P. T. (2010). Pooled energy budgets: resituating human energy allocation tradeoffs. Evolutionary Anthropology, 19, 136–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lahdenperä, M., Lummaa, V., Helle, S., Tremblay, M., & Russell, A. F. (2004). Fitness benefits of prolonged post-reproductive lifespan in women. Nature, 428, 178–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lancaster, J. B., & Lancaster, C. S. (1983). Parental investment: The hominid adaptation. In D. Ortner (Ed.), How humans adapt (pp. 35–56). Washington: Smithsonian Institution Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lancy, D. F. (2007). Accounting for variability in mother–child play. American Anthropologist, 109(2), 273–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leavens, D. A., Hopkins, W. D., & Bard, K. A. (2008). The heterochronic origins of explicit reference. In J. Zlatev, T. P. Racine, C. Sinha, & E. Itkonen (Eds.), The shared mind: Perspectives on intersubjectivity. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leavens, D. A., Bard, K. A., & Hopkins, W. D. (2010). BIZARRE chimpanzees do not represent ‘The Chimpanzee’. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2-1), 100–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leavitt, S. D., & List, J. A. (2007). What do laboratory experiments measuring social preferences reveal about the real world? Journal of Economic Perspectives, 21(2), 153–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, R. D. (2003). Rethinking the evolutionary theory of aging: transfers, not births, shape social species. Proceedings of the National Academy Sciences USA, 100, 9637–9642.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, R. D. (2008). Sociality, selection, and survival: simulated evolution of mortality with intergenerational transfers and food sharing. Proceedings of the National Academy Sciences USA, 105, 7124–7128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leigh, S. R., & Park, P. B. (1998). Evolution of human growth prolongation. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 107, 331–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, M., Sullivan, M. W., Stanger, C., & Weiss, M. (1989). Self development and self-conscious emotions. Child Development, 60(1), 146–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lin, A. C., Bard, K. A., & Anderson, J. R. (1992). Development of self-recognition in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 106(2), 120–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, C. H., & Tronick, E. (2011). Neurobehavioural development in infancy. In D. Skuse, H. Bruce, L. Dowdney, & D. Mrazek (Eds.), Child psychology and psychiatry: Frameworks for practice (2nd ed., pp. 14–22). New York: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lovejoy, C. O. (1981). The origin of man. Science, 2011(4480), 341–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mahajan, N., & Wynn, K. (2012). Origins of “Us” versus “Them”: prelinguistic infants prefer similar others. Cognition, 124, 227–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marlowe, F. W. (2003). A critical period for provisioning by Hadza men: implications for pair bonding. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 217–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marlowe, F. W. (2004). Marital residence among Foragers. Current Anthropology, 45, 277–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matsuzawa, T. (2012). What is uniquely human? A view from comparative cognitive development in humans and chimpanzees. In F. B. M. de Waal & P. F. Ferrari (Eds.), The primate mind (pp. 288–305). Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McKinney, M. L., & McNamara, K. J. (1991). Heterochrony: The evolution of ontogeny. New York: Plenum Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Melis, A. P., & Semmann, D. (2010). How is human cooperation different? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B: Biological Sciences, 365, 2663–2674.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Melis, A., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Engineering cooperation in chimpanzees: tolerance constraints on cooperation. Animal Behavior, 72, 275–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Minugh-Purvis, N., & McNamara, K. J. (Eds.). (2002). Human evolution through developmental change. Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Montague, A. (1961). Neonatal and infant immaturity in man. Journal of the American Medical Association, 178(1), 156–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Myowa-Yamakoshi, M., Tomonaga, M., Tanaka, M., & Matsuzawa, T. (2004). Imitation in neonatal chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Developmental Science, 7, 437–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagell, K., Olguin, R. S., & Tomasello, M. (1993). Processes of social learning in the tool use of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 107, 174–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, E. (2008). Infant intersubjectivity: newborns’ sensitivity to communication disturbance. Developmental Psychology, 44(6), 1779–1784.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, M., & Tomaselli, K. (2010). Overimitation in Kalahari Bushman children and the origins of human cultural cognition. Psychological Sciences, 21, 729–736.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Connell, J. F., Hawkes, K., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (1999). Grandmothering and the evolution of Homo erectus. Journal of Human Evolution, 36, 461–485.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parker, S. T., Langer, J., & McKinney, M. L. (Eds.). (2000). Biology, brains, and behavior: The evolution of human development. Santa Fe: SAR Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Penn, D. C., & Povinelli, D. J. (2007). Causal cognition in human and nonhuman animals: a comparative, critical review. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 97–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Penn, D. C., Holyoak, K. J., & Povinelli, D. (2008). Darwin’s mistake: explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 109–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plooij, F. X. (1984). The behavioral development of free-living chimpanzee babies and infants. Norwood: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Povinelli, D. J. (2000). Folk physics for apes: The chimpanzee’s theory of how the world works. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Povinelli, D. J., & Vonk, J. (2003). Chimpanzee minds: suspiciously human? Trends in Cognitive Science, 7(4), 157–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Puts, D. A. (2010). Beauty and the beast: mechanisms of sexual selection in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31, 151–175.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reddy, V. (2003). On being an object of attention: implications for self-other-consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Science, 7(9), 397–402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reddy, V. (2008). How infants know minds. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosati, A. G., Santos, L. R., & Hare, B. (2010). Primate social cognition: Thirty years after Premack and Woodruff. In M. L. Platt & A. A. Ghanzanfar (Eds.), Primate neuroethology (pp. 117–143). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Ruiz, A. M., & Santos, L. (2012). What does the primate mind know about other minds? A review of primates’ understanding of visual attention. In F. B. M. de Waal & P. F. Ferrari (Eds.), The primate mind (pp. 139–157). Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sakai, T., Mikami, A., Tomonaga, M., Matsui, M., Suzuki, J., Hamada, Y., et al. (2011). Differential prefrontal white matter development in chimpanzees and humans. Current Biology, 21(16), 1397–1402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scarf, D., Imuta, K., Colombo, M., & Hayne, H. (2012). Social evaluation or simple association? Simple associations may explain moral reasoning in infants. PLoS ONE, 7(8), e42698.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmelz, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Chimpanzees know that others make inferences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 108(7), 3077–3079.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schultz, A. H. (1969). The life of primates. New York: Universe Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sear, R., & Coall, D. (2011). How much does family matter? Cooperative breeding and the demographic transition. Popullation and Development Reviews, 37, 81–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sear, R., & Mace, R. (2008). Who keeps children alive? A review of the effects of kin on child survival. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (2007). Primate social knowledge and the origins of language. Mind and Society, 7(1), 129–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (2012). The evolutionary origins of friendship. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 153–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shea, B. T. (1989). Heterochrony in human evolution: the case for neotony reconsidered. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology, 32, 69–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheskin, M., & Santos, L. (2012). The evolution of morality: Which aspects of human moral concerns are shared with nonhuman primates? In J. Vonk & T. K. Shackelford (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of comparative evolutionary psychology (pp. 434–449). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shettleworth, S. J. (2010). Clever animals and killjoy explanations in comparative psychology. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(11), 477–481.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silk, J. B. (2009). Forum. In M. Tomasello (Ed.), Why we cooperate (pp. 111–122). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silk, J. B., & House, B. R. (2011). Evolutionary foundations of human prosocial sentiments. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 108(Suppl 2), 10910–10917.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sober, E., & Wilson, D. S. (1998). Unto others: The evolution and psychology of unselfish behavior. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Somel, M., Franz, H., Yan, Z., Lorenc, A., Guo, S., et al. (2009). Transcriptional neoteny in the human brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 106, 5743–5748.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Somel, M., Tang, L., & Khaitovich, P. (2012). The role of neoteny in human evolution: from genes to the phenotype. In H. Hirai, H. Imai, & Y. Go (Eds.), Post-Genome Biology of Primates, Primatology Monographs, Part 1, 23–41. New York: Springer.

  • Sommerville, J. A., Woodward, A. L., & Needham, A. (2005). Action experience alters 3-month-old infants’ perception of others’ actions. Cognition, 96, B1–B11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spelke, E. S. (2009). Forum. In M. Tomasello (Ed.), Why we cooperate (pp. 149–172). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spelke, E. S., & Kinzler, K. D. (2007). Core knowledge. Developmental Science, 10, 89–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Super, C. (1981). Behavioral development in infancy. In R. H. Monroe, R. L. Monroe, & B. B. Whiting (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural human development (pp. 181–270). New York: Garland STPM Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Takeshita, H., Myowa-Yamakoshi, M., & Satoshi, H. (2009). The supine position of postnatal human infants: Implications for the development of cognitive intelligence. Interaction Studies, 10(2), 252–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, J. L., Krovitz, G. E., & Nelson, A. J. (Eds.) (2003). Patterns of growth and development in the genus Homo. Cambridge Studies in Biological and Evolutionary Anthropology 37. Cambridge; Cambridge University Press.

  • Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge: Harvard Univ. Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2008). Origins of human communication. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2009). Why we cooperate. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (2010). Chimpanzee social cognition. In E. V. Lonsdorf, S. R. Ross, & T. Matsuzawa (Eds.), The mind of the chimpanzee (pp. 235–250). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2007). Shared intentionality. Developmental Science, 10(1), 121–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Haberl, K. (2003). Understanding attention: 12- and 18-month-olds know what is new for other persons. Developmental Psychology, 39(5), 906–912.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Herrmann, E. (2010). Ape and human cognition: what’s the difference? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19, 3–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Rakocszy, H. (2007). The ontogeny of social ontology: Steps to shared intentionality and status functions. In S. L. Tsohatzidis (Ed.), Intentional acts and institutional facts: Essays on John Searle’s social ontology (pp. 113–137). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675–735.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Melis, A., Tennie, C., Wyman, E., & Herrmann, E. (2012). Two key steps in the evolution of human cooperation: the mutualism hypothesis. Current Anthropology, 53(6), 673–692.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomonaga, M., Tanaka, M., Matsuzawa, T., Myowa-Yamakoshi, M., Kosugi, D., Mizuno, Y., et al. (2004). Development of social cognition in infant chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): face recognition, smiling, gaze, and the lack of triadic interactions. Japanese Psychological Research, 46, 227–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy: A descriptive of primary intersubjectivity. In M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before speech: The beginnings of interpersonal communication (pp. 321–347). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C., & Aitken, K. J. (2001). Infant intersubjectivity: research, theory, and clinical applications. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 3–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bard, K. A., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Ivan, K. (2009). Enhancement of attachment and cognitive development of young nursery-reared chimpanzees in responsive versus standard care. Developmental Psychobiology, 51, 173–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Lawick-Goodall, J. (1968). The behaviour of free-living chimpanzees of the Gombe Stream Area. Animal Behavior Monographs, 1, 161–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Voland, E., Chasiotis, A., & Schiefenhovel, W. (Eds.). (2005). Grandmotherhood: The evolutionary significance of the second half of female life. New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Washburn, S. L., & Lancaster, C. S. (1968). The evolution of hunting. In R. B. Lee & I. DeVore (Eds.), Man the hunter (pp. 293–303). New York: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • West-Eberhard, M. J. (2003). Developmental plasticity and evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiessner, P. (2009). Experimental games and games of life among the Ju/’hoan Bushmen. Current Anthropology, 50(1), 133–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilkins, J. F., & Marlowe, F. W. (2006). Sex-biased migration in humans: what should we expect from genetic data? BioEssays, 28, 290–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, G. C. (1966). Adaptation and natural selection: A critique of some current evolutionary thought. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, E. O. (2012). The social conquest of earth. New York: Liveright, Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wobber, V., Wrangham, R., & Hare, B. (2010). Bonobos exhibit delayed development of social behavior and cognition relative to chimpanzees. Current Biology, 20, 226–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodward, A. L. (1999). Infants’ ability to distinguish between purposeful and non-purposeful behaviors. Infant Behavior and Development, 22(2), 145–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wrangham, R. (2010). Meanings of chimpanzee mind. In E. V. Lonsdorf, S. R. Ross, & T. Matsuzawa (Eds.), The mind of the chimpanzee (pp. 370–374). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wroblewski, E. E. (2008). An unusual incident of adoption in a wild chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) population at Gombe National Park. American Journal of Primatology, 70(10), 995–998. doi:10.1002/ajp.20582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wynn, K. (2008). Some innate foundations of social and moral cognition. In P. Carruthers, S. Laurence, & S. Stich (Eds.), The innate mind (Foundations and the Future, Vol. 3, pp. 330–347). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I thank especially Nick Blurton Jones and Sarah Hrdy, as well as Ted Coxworth, Steve Beckerman, Karen Kramer, Jim O’Connell, and four anonymous reviewers for helpful comments and good advice.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kristen Hawkes.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hawkes, K. Primate Sociality to Human Cooperation. Hum Nat 25, 28–48 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-013-9184-x

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-013-9184-x

Keywords

Navigation