Skip to main content
Log in

Convergent cognition

  • Published:
Instructional Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In an attempt to address shortcomings revealed in international assessments and lamented in legislation, many schools are reducing or eliminating elective courses, applying the rationale that replacing “non-essential” subjects with core subjects, such as mathematics and language arts, will better position students in the global market. However, there is evidence that systematically pairing a core subject with another, complementary subject, may lead to greater overall learning in both subjects. In this paper, we analyze two subject area pairs—first and second language, and computer programming and mathematics—to demonstrate in what ways two subjects might complement each other. We then analyze the relationships between these pairs to better understand the principles and conditions that encourage what we call convergent cognition, the synergistic effect that occurs when a learner studies two complementary subjects.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Agalianos, A., Noss, R., & Whitty, G. (2001). Logo in mainstream schools: The struggle over the soul of an educational innovation. British Journal of Sociology of Education, 22(4), 479–500.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • American Council of Teachers of Foreign Languages. (2009). What does research show about the benefits of language learning? Retrieved December 12, 2009 from http://www.actfl.org/i4a/pages/index.cfm?pageid=4724.

  • Andrade, C. (1989). Two languages for all children: Expanding to low achievers and the handicapped. In E. Muller (Ed.), Languages in the elementary schools (pp. 177–203). New York: The American Forum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Austin, H. S. (1987). Predictors of Pascal programming achievement for community college students. In Proceedings of the Eighteenth SIGCSE Technical Symposium on Computer Science Education (pp. 161–164). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

  • Bae, J. (2007). Development of English skills need not suffer as a result of immersion: Grades 1 and 2 writing assessment in a Korean/English two-way immersion program. Language Learning, 57(2), 299–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barber, G. E. (1985). Latin as a practical study. The Classical Journal, 81(2), 158–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnett, S. M., & Ceci, S. J. (2002). When and where do we apply what we learn?: A taxonomy for far transfer. Psychological Bulletin, 128(4), 612–637.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bassok, M., & Holyoak, K. J. (1989). Interdomain transfer between isomorphic topics in algebra and physics. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15(1), 153–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bassok, M., & Holyoak, K. J. (1993). Pragmatic knowledge and conceptual structure: Determinants of transfer between quantitative domains. Ablex Publishing.

  • Bastian, T. R. (1980). An investigation into the effects of second language learning on achievement in English. Dissertation Abstracts International. 40(12-A, Pt. 1), 6176–6177.

  • Bergin, S., & Reilly, R. (2006). Predicting introductory programming performance: A multi-institutional multivariate study. Computer Science Education, 16(4), 303–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bloch, S. (2007). Teach scheme!: A functional-first approach to beginning programming. Journal of Computing Sciences in Colleges, 22(3), 161–163.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, P., & Lyons, G. (2001). The effect of student attributes on success in programming. ACM SIGCSE Bulletin, 33(3), 49–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • California dept. of Education. (2010). Student access to core courses. Retrieved October 1, 2010 from http://www.cde.ca.gov/ta/lp/vl/hsepc2.asp.

  • Campione, J. C., & Brown, A. L. (1981). Inducing flexible thinking: The problem of access. In M. P. Friedman, J. P. Das, & N. O’Connor (Eds.), Intelligence and learning. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campione, J. C., & Brown, A. L. (1984). Learning ability and transfer propensity as sources of individual differences in intelligence. In Learning and Cognition in the Mentally Retarded (pp. 265–293). Hillsdale: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates.

  • Carraher, D., & Schliemann, A. (2002). The transfer dilemma. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 11(1), 1–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, X., Xu, F., Nguyen, T. K., Hong, G., & Wang, Y. (2010). Effects of cross-language transfer on first-language phonological awareness and literacy skills in Chinese children receiving English instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(3), 712–728. doi:10.1037/a0018802.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cisero, C. A., & Royer, J. M. (1995). The development and cross-language transfer of phonological awareness. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 20(3), 275–303. doi:10.1006/ceps.1995.1018.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clements, D. H., Battista, M. T., & Sarama, J. (2001). Logo and geometry [Monograph]. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 42(2), 127–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collier, V. P., & Thomas, W. P. (2002). The astounding effectiveness of dual language education for all. NABE Journal of Research and Practice, 2(1), 1–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, V. (1992). Evidence of multicompetence. Language Learning, 42, 557–591.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corte, E. D., Verschaffel, L., & Schrooten, H. (1992). Computer-based learning environments and problem solving (pp. 207–228). New York: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Cummins, J. (1978). The cognitive development of children in immersion programs. Canadian Modern Language Review, 34(5), 855–883.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, T. H., & Graham, C. R. (2000). Increasing native English vocabulary recognition through Spanish immersion: Cognate transfer from foreign to first language. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(1), 37–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Corte, E., & Verschaffel, L. (1986). Effects of computer experience on children’s thinking skills. Journal of Structural Learning, 9, 161–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Corte, E., & Verschaffel, L. (1989). Logo, a vehicle for thinking. In B. Greer & G. Mulhern (Eds.), New directions in mathematics education (pp. 63–81). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deary, I., Strand, S., Smith, P., & Fernandes, C. (2007). Intelligence and educational achievement. Intelligence, 35(1), 13–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Demie, F., & Strand, S. (2006). English language acquisition and educational attainment at the end of secondary school. Educational Studies, 32(2), 215–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doye, P. (1977). Untersuchungen zum englischunterricht in der grundschule: Bericht uber D. Westermann: Braunschweiger forschungsprojekt FEU.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eddy, P. A. (1981). The effect of foreign language study in high school on verbal ability as measured by the scholastic aptitude test-verbal. Final report. Washington, D.C.: Center for applied linguistics. (ERIC Document Reproduction Services No. ED 196 312).

  • Feurzeig, W., Papert, S., Bloom, M., Grant, R., & Solomon, C. (1970). Programming-languages as a conceptual framework for teaching mathematics. SIGCUE Outlook, 4(2), 13–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friel, B. (2003). Don’t know much about history. National Journal, 35(31), 2550–2551.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gagné, R. M. (1974). Essentials of learning for instruction. Hinsdale: The Dryden Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gale, K., McClay, D., Christie, M., & Harris, S. (1981). Academic achievement in the Milingimbi bilingual education program. TESOL Quarterly, 15(3), 297–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gorman, H., & Bourne, L. E. (1983). Learning to think by learning logo: Rule learning in third-grade computer programmers. Bulletin of Psychonomic Society, 21, 165–167.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gruber, H. E., & Gruber, V. (1962). The eye of reason: Darwin’s development during the beagle voyage. Isis, 53, 186–200.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haskell, R. E. (2001). Transfer of learning: Cognition, instruction, and reasoning (illustrated ed.). San Diego: Academic Press.

  • Honour-Werth, L. (1986). Predicting student performance in a beginning computer science class. In Proceedings of the Seventeenth SIGCSE Technical Symposium on Computer Science Education (pp. 138–143). Cincinnati: SIGCSE.

  • Hostetler, T. R. (1983). Predicting student success in an introductory programming course. ACM SIGCSE Bulletin, 15(3), 40–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howard, E. R., Sugarman, J., & Christian, D. (2003). Trends in two-way immersion education: A review of the research (report no. 63). Baltimore: Center for Research on the Education of Students Placed at Risk. Retrieved from April 19, 2007.

  • Hoyles, C., & Noss, R. (1987). Synthesizing mathematical conceptions and their formalization through the construction of a logo-based school mathematics curriculum. International Journal of Mathematical Education in Science and Technology, 18(4), 581–595.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoyles, C., & Noss, R. (1992). Learning mathematics and logo. Cambridge: MIT Press.

  • Jarvis, S., & Pavlenko, A. (2008). Crosslinguistic influence in language and cognition (illustrated ed.). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, D. C. (2000). Algorithmics and programming in the school mathematics curriculum: Support is waning—is there still a case to be made? Education and Information Technologies, 5, 201–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kesckes, I., & Papp, T. (2000). Foreign language and mother tongue. Hillsdale: Lawrence Earlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knuth, E. J., Alibali, M. W., McNeil, N. M., Weinberg, A., & Stephens, A. C. (2005). Middle school students’ understanding of core algebraic concepts: Equivalence & variable 1. ZDM, 37(1), 68–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kotovsky, K., Hayes, J. R., & Simon, H. A. (1985). Why are some problems hard? Evidence from the tower of Hanoi. Cognitive Psychology, 17, 248–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krashen, S. D. (1987). English language teaching series: Principles and practices in second language acquisition. London: Prentice-Hall: International (UK) Ltd.

  • Leatham, K. R. (2006). Viewing mathematics teachers’ beliefs as sensible systems. Journal of Mathematics Teacher Education, 9(1), 91–102. doi:10.1007/s10857-006-9006-8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leeper, R. R., & Silver, J. L. (1982). Predicting success in a first programming course. In Proceedings of the Thirteenth SIGCSE Technical Symposium on Computer Science Education, (pp. 147–150). Dallas: SIGCSE.

  • Leinhart, G., Zalavsky, O., & Stein, M. K. (1990). Functions graphs, and graphing. Tasks, Learning, and Teaching, 60(1), 1–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Linn, M. C., & Dalbey, J. (1985). Cognitive consequences of programming instruction, access, and ability. Educational Psychologist, 20, 191–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lopato, E. (1963). FLES and academic achievement. The French Review, 36(5), 499–507.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manis, F. R., Lindsey, K. A., & Bailey, C. E. (2004). Development of reading in grades K-2 in Spanish-speaking English-language learners. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 19(4), 214–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Masciantonio, R. (1977). Tangible benefits of the study of Latin: A review of research. Foreign Language Annals, 10(4), 375–382.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCoy, L. P. (1988). General variable skill, computer programming and mathematics. In Paper presented at the annual meeting of the International Association for Computing in Education. New Orlean: IACE.

  • McCoy, L. (1996). Computer-Based mathematics learning. Journal of Research on Computing in Education, 28, 438–460.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, R., Kelly, G., & Kelly, J. (1988). Effects of logo computer programming experiences on problem solving and spatial relations ability. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 13, 348–357.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milner, S. (1973). The effects of computer programming on performance in mathematics. In Proceedings from the annual meeting of the american educational research association. New Orlean, LA.

  • Moreno, R., Ozogul, G., & Reisslein, M. (2011). Teaching with concrete and abstract visual representations: Effects on students’ problem solving, problem representations, and learning perceptions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(1), 32. doi:10.1037/a0021995.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (2000). Principles and standards for school mathematics. Reston: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • North, K. (2007). Integrated algebra/computer science. Retrieved December 12, 2009 from http://teachertech.rice.edu/Participants/knorth/algebra/index.htm.

  • Noss, R. (1986). Constructing a conceptual framework for elementary algebra through logo programming. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 17(4), 335–357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • OECD. (2007). PISA 2006: Science competencies for tomorrow’s world. Exectuve summary. Paris: OECD.

  • Offenberg, R. M. (1971). Evaluation of the elementary school (FLES) Latin program 1970–1971. Philadelphia: Philadelphia School District Office. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service NO. ED 056 612).

  • Oprea, J. M. (1988). Computer programming and mathematical thinking. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 7(2), 175–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palumbo, D. B. (1990). Programming language/problem-solving research: A review of relevant issues. Review of Educational Research, 60(1), 65–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paris, S. G., & McEvoy, A. P. (2000). Harmful and enduring effects of high-stakes testing. Issues in Education, 6(1/2), 145–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pavlenko, A. (2003). ‘I feel clumsy speaking Russian’: L2 influence on L1 in narratives of Russian L2 users of English. In Second language acquisition: Effects of the second language on the first (pp. 32–61). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.

  • Paz, T., & Leron, U. (2009). The slippery road from actions on objects to functions and variables. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 40, 18–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pea, R. D., & Kurland, D. M. (1984). On the cognitive effects of learning computer programming. New Ideas in Psychology, 2(2), 137–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. New York: Norton.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Powell, A. G., Farrar, E., & Cohen, D. K. (1985). The shopping mall high school: Winners and losers in the educational marketplace. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

  • Rosenthal, B. (2004). No subject left behind? Think again. NEA Today, 23(2), 26–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Säljö, R. (2003). Epilogue: From transfer to boundary crossing new perspectives on transfer and boundary-crossing between school and work (p. 311). New York: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salomon, G., & Perkins, D. (1989). Rocky roads to transfer: Rethinking mechanisms of a neglected phenomenon. Educational Psychologist, 24, 113–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, B. B., & Hershkowitz, R. (1999). Prototypes: Brakes or levers in learning the function concept? The role of computer tools. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 30, 362–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheridan, R. (1976). Augmenting reading skills through language learning transfer. FLES latin program evaluation reports, 1973–74, 1974–75, 1975–76 [microform]/rita Sheridan.

  • Sinclair, N. (2008). The history of the geometry curriculum in the United States. Charlotte: Information Age Publishing, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Soloway, E., Lochhead, J., & Clement, J. (1982). Does computer programming enhance problem-solving ability? In R. E. Seidel, R. E. Anderson, & B. Hunter (Eds.), Computer literacy (pp. 171–185). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21(4), 360–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stewart, J. H. (2005). Foreign language study in elementary schools: Benefits and implications for achievement in reading and math. Early Childhood Education Journal, 33(1), 11–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Subhi, T. (1999). The impact of LOGO on gifted children’s achievement and creativity. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 15(2), 98–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, P. W. (1994). Students, functions, and the undergraduate curriculum. In E. Dubinsky, A. H. Schoenfeld, & J. J. Kaput (Eds.), Research in collegiate mathematics education (pp. 21–44). Providence: American Mathematical Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. L., & Woodworth, R. S. (1901). The influence of improvement in one mental function upon the efficiency of other functions. Psychological Review, 8, 247–261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turnbull, M., Lapkin, S., & Hart, D. (2001). Grade 3 immersion students’ performance in literacy and mathematics: Province-wide results from Ontario (1998–1999). Canadian Modern Language Review, 58(1), 9–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • U.S. Department of Education. (2008). Foundations for succes. The final report of the national mathematics advisory panel. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education.

  • Usiskin, Z. (1988). Conceptions of school algebra and uses of variables. In The ideas of algebra. Virginia: The National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, Inc.

  • Weatherford, H. J. (1986). Personal benefits of foreign language study. Eric digest # 276 305. Washington, D.C.: Office of Educational Research and Improvement.

  • Wertheimer, M. (1959). Productive thinking. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, B. C., & Shrock, S. (2001). Contributing to success in an introductory computer science course: A study of twelve factors. ACM SIGCSE Bulletin, 33(1), 184–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zheng, R. (2009). Cognitive effects of multimedia learning (illustrated ed.). Hershey: Idea Group Inc (IGI).

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Peter J. Rich.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Rich, P.J., Leatham, K.R. & Wright, G.A. Convergent cognition. Instr Sci 41, 431–453 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-012-9240-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-012-9240-7

Keywords

Navigation