Skip to main content
Log in

Auditory processing, linguistic prosody awareness, and word reading in Mandarin-speaking children learning English

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study examined language-specific links among auditory processing, linguistic prosody awareness, and Mandarin (L1) and English (L2) word reading in 61 Mandarin-speaking, English-learning children. Three auditory discrimination abilities were measured: pitch contour, pitch interval, and rise time (rate of intensity change at tone onset). Linguistic prosody awareness was measured three ways: Mandarin tone perception, English stress perception, and English stress production. A Chinese character recognition task was the Mandarin L1 reading metric. English L2 word reading was assessed by English real word reading and nonword decoding tasks. The importance of the auditory processing measures to reading was different in the two languages. Pitch contour discrimination predicted Mandarin L1 word reading and rise time discrimination predicted English L2 word reading, after controlling for age and nonverbal IQ. For the prosodic and phonological measures, Mandarin tone perception, but not rhyme awareness, predicted Chinese character recognition after controlling for age and nonverbal IQ. In contrast, English rhyme awareness predicted more unique variance in English real word reading and nonword decoding than did English stress perception and production. Linguistic prosody awareness appears to play a more important role in L1 word reading than phonological awareness; while the reverse seems true for English L2 word reading in Mandarin-speaking children. Taken together, auditory processing, language-specific linguistic prosody awareness, and phonological awareness play different roles in L1/L2 reading, reflecting different prosodic and segmental structures between Mandarin and English.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Antoniou, M., To, C. K. S., & Wong, P. C. M. (2015). Auditory cues that drive language development are language specific: Evidence from Cantonese. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36, 1493–1507. doi:10.1017/S0142716414000514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arciuli, J., & Cupples, L. (2006). The processing of lexical stress during visual word recognition: Typicality effects and orthographic correlates. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 920–948. doi:10.1080/02724980443000782.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bidelman, G. M., & Chung, W.-L. (2015). Tone-language speakers show hemispheric specialization and differential cortical processing of contour and interval cues for pitch. Neuroscience, 305, 384–392. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.08.010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bidelman, G. M., Gandour, J. T., & Krishnan, A. (2011). Cross-domain effects of music and language experience on the representation of pitch in the human auditory brainstem. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 425–434. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowey, J. A., Cain, M. T., & Ryan, S. M. (1992). A reading-level design study of phonological skills underlying fourth-grade children’s word reading difficulties. Child Development, 63, 999–1011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chan, I.-C., Hu, C.-F., & Wan, I.-P. (2005). Learning to read and spell: The relative role of phonemic awareness and onset-rime awareness. Taiwan Journal of Linguistics, 3(1), 65–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, H., & Chen, R. (2006). Raven’s standard progressive matrices. Taipei: Chinese Behavioral Science Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chiat, S. (1983). Why Mikey’s right and my key’s wrong: The significance of stress and word boundaries in a child’s output system. Cognition, 14(3), 275–300. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(83)90007-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Choi, J.-Y., Hasegawa-Johnson, M., & Cole, J. (2005). Finding intonational boundaries using acoustic cues related to the voice source. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 118, 2579–2587. doi:10.1121/1.2010288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chung, W.-L., & Bidelman, G. M. (2016). Cortical encoding and neurophysiological tracking of intensity and pitch cues signaling English stress patterns in native and nonnative speakers. Brain and Language, 155–156, 49–57. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2016.04.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chung, W.-L., & Hu, C.-F. (2007). Morphological awareness and learning to read Chinese. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 20, 441–461. doi:10.1007/s11145-006-9037-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chung, W.-L., & Jarmulowicz, L. (2017). Stress judgment and production in English derivation, and word reading in adult Mandarin-speaking English learners. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research. doi:10.1007/s10936-017-9475-1.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cicchetti, D. V. (1994). Guidelines, criteria, and rules of thumb for evaluating normed and standardized assessment instruments in psychology. Psychological Assessment, 6, 284–290. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.6.4.284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Common Core State Standard Initiative. (2015). Reading: Foundational skills. Retrieved from http://www.corestandards.org/ELA-Literacy/RF/K.

  • Corriveau, K., Pasquini, E., & Goswami, U. (2007). Basic auditory processing skills and specific language impairment: A new look at an old hypothesis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 647–666. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2007/046).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, A. (1996). Prosody and the word boundary problem. In J. L. Morgan & K. Demuth (Eds.), Signal to syntax: Bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition (pp. 87–99). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dowling, W. J. (1982). Melodic information processing and its development. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), The Psychology of Music (pp. 413–429). New York: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Duanmu, S. (2000). The phonology of Standard Chinese. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Echols, C. H. (1996). A role for stress in early speech segmentation. In J. L. Morgan & K. Demuth (Eds.), Signal to syntax: Bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition (pp. 151–170). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foxton, J. M., Talcott, J. B., Witton, C., Brace, H., McIntyre, F., & Griffiths, T. D. (2003). Reading skills are related to global, but not local, acoustic pattern perception. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 343–344. doi:10.1038/nn1035.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fry, D. B. (1958). Experiments in the perception of stress. Language and Speech, 1(2), 126–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U., Gerson, D., & Astruc, L. (2010). Amplitude envelope perception, phonology and prosodic sensitivity in children with developmental dyslexia. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 23, 995–1019.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U., & Leong, V. (2013). Speech rhythm and temporal structure: Converging perspectives. Laboratory Phonology, 4, 67–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U., Mead, N., Fosker, T., Huss, M., Barnes, L., & Leong, V. (2013). Impaired perception of syllable stress in children with dyslexia: A longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 69(1), 1–17. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2013.03.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U., Wang, H.-L. S., Cruz, A., Fosker, T., Mead, N., & Huss, M. (2011). Language-universal sensory deficits in developmental dyslexia: English, Spanish, and Chinese. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 325–337. doi:10.1162/jocn.2010.21453.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, S. (1999). Speaking in shorthand—A syllable-centric perspective for understanding pronunciation variation. Speech Communication, 29(2–4), 159–176. doi:10.1016/S0167-6393(99)00050-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hallgren, K. A. (2012). Computing inter-rater reliability for observational data: An overview and tutorial. Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 8, 23–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holliman, A. J., Wood, C., & Sheehy, K. (2008). Sensitivity to speech rhythm explains individual differences in reading ability independently of phonological awareness. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 26, 357–367. doi:10.1348/026151007X241623.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howie, J. M. (1976). Acoustical studies of Mandarin vowels and tones. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hu, C.-F. (2013). Predictors of reading in children with Chinese as a first language: A developmental and cross-linguistic perspective. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 26, 163–187. doi:10.1007/s11145-012-9360-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hu, C.-F., & Catts, H. W. (1998). The role of phonological processing in early reading ability: What we can learn from Chinese. Scientific Studies of Reading, 2, 55–79. doi:10.1207/s1532799xssr0201_3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hu, C.-F., & Schuele, C. M. (2005). Learning nonnative names: The effect of poor native phonological awareness. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 343–362. doi:10.1017/S0142716405050204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huang, H. S. (2004). Graded Chinese character recognition test. Taipein: Psychological Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jarmulowicz, L., Taran, V. L., & Hay, S. E. (2007). Third graders’ metalinguistic skills, reading skills, and stress production in derived English words. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 1593–1605. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2007/107).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kehoe, M., Stoel-Gammon, C., & Buder, E. H. (1995). Acoustic correlates of stress in young children’s speech. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 338–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kitzen, K. R. (2001). Prosodic sensitivity, morphological ability, and reading ability in young adults with and without childhood histories of reading difficulty. Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database (UMI No. 3005743).

  • Kochanski, G., Grabe, E., Coleman, J., & Rosner, B. (2005). Loudness predicts prominence: Fundamental frequency lends little. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 118, 1038–1054.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krishnan, A., Gandour, J. T., & Bidelman, G. M. (2010). The effects of tone language experience on pitch processing in the brainstem. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 23(1), 81–95. doi:10.1016/j.jneuroling.2009.09.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levitt, H. (1971). Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 49, 467–477. doi:10.1121/1.1912375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Li, H., Shu, H., McBride-Chang, C., Liu, H., & Peng, H. (2012). Chinese children’s character recognition: Visuo-orthographic, phonological processing and morphological skills. Journal of Research in Reading, 35, 287–307. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9817.2010.01460.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Li, C. N., & Thompson, S. A. (1981). Mandarin Chinese: A functional reference grammar. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liu, Y.-H., & Hu, C.-F. (2010). The role of Chinese EFL learners’ sensitivity to English lexical stress patterns in grammatical category assignments. English Teaching and Learning, 34(4), 1–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorenzi, C., Dumont, A., & Füllgrabe, C. (2000). Use of temporal envelope cues by children with developmental dyslexia. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 1367–1379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., Tong, X., Shu, H., Wong, A. M.-Y., Leung, K., & Tardif, T. (2008). Syllable, phoneme, and tone: Psycholinguistic units in early Chinese and English word recognition. Scientific Studies of Reading, 12, 171–194. doi:10.1080/10888430801917290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mehta, G., & Cutler, A. (1988). Detection of target phonemes in spontaneous and read speech. Language and Speech, 31, 135–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ministry of Education. (2002). Report on frequent characters and words in elementary school children. Taipei: Ministry of Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morton, J., & Jassem, W. (1965). Acoustic correlates of stress. Language and Speech, 8, 159–181. doi:10.1177/002383096500800303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M., & Taylor, S. (1998). Segmentation, not rhyming, predicts early progress in learning to read. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 71(1), 3–27. doi:10.1006/jecp.1998.2453.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ou, S.-C. (2010). Taiwanese EFL learners’ perception of English word stress. Concentric: Studies in Linguistics, 36(1), 1–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patel, A. D., Peretz, I., Tramo, M., & Labreque, R. (1998). Processing prosodic and musical patterns: A neuropsychological investigation. Brain and Language, 61, 123–144. doi:10.1006/brln.1997.1862.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pierrehumbert, J. B. (2003). Phonetic diversity, statistical learning, and acquisition of phonology. Language and Speech, 46, 115–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siok, W. T., & Fletcher, P. (2001). The role of phonological awareness and visual-orthographic skills in Chinese reading acquisition. Developmental Psychology, 37, 886–899.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • So, D., & Siegel, L. S. (1997). Learning to read Chinese: Semantic, syntactic, phonological and working memory skills in normally achieving and poor Chinese readers. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 9, 1–21. doi:10.1023/A:1007963513853.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (2012). Test of word reading efficiency-II. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vihman, M., & Croft, W. (2007). Phonological development: Toward a “radical” templatic phonology. Linguistics, 45, 683–725. doi:10.1515/LING.2007.021.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, H.-L. S., Huss, M., Hämäläinen, J. A., & Goswami, U. (2012). Basic auditory processing and developmental dyslexia in Chinese. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 25, 509–536. doi:10.1007/s11145-010-9284-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whalley, K., & Hansen, J. (2006). The role of prosodic sensitivity in children’s reading development. Journal of Research in Reading, 29, 288–303. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9817.2006.00309.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, C., & Terrell, C. (1998). Poor readers’ ability to detect speech rhythm and perceive rapid speech. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 397–413. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.1998.tb00760.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yu, V. Y., & Andruski, J. E. (2010). A cross-language study of perception of lexical stress in English. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 39, 323–344. doi:10.1007/s10936-009-9142-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, J., & McBride-Chang, C. (2010). Auditory sensitivity, speech perception, and reading development and impairment. Educational Psychology Review, 22, 323–338. doi:10.1007/s10648-010-9137-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, J., & McBride-Chang, C. (2014). Auditory sensitivity, speech perception, L1 Chinese, and L2 English reading abilities in Hong Kong Chinese children. Developmental Psychology, 50, 1001–1013. doi:10.1037/a0035086.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, Y., Nissen, S. L., & Francis, A. L. (2008). Acoustic characteristics of English lexical stress produced by native Mandarin speakers. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 123, 4498–4513. doi:10.1121/1.2902165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Wei-Lun Chung.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Chung, WL., Jarmulowicz, L. & Bidelman, G.M. Auditory processing, linguistic prosody awareness, and word reading in Mandarin-speaking children learning English. Read Writ 30, 1407–1429 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9730-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9730-8

Keywords

Navigation