Skip to main content
Log in

The influence of reading unit size on the development of Stroop interference in early word decoding

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The purpose of the experiments was to determine the automatic use of large or small word reading units in young readers in the absence of word decoding strategies. Picture-word Stroop interference was examined from four types of conflicting labels: (a) words containing both highly predictable grapheme–phoneme correspondence (GPC) units and highly consistent rime units (henceforth, Hi-GPC + Hi-Rime); (b) words with highly predictable GPC units and less consistent rime units (Hi-GPC + Lo-Rime); (c) words with low predictability GPC units and highly consistent rime units (Low GPC + High Rime); (d) nonwords that contained both highly predictable GPC and highly consistent rime units. Naming time for pictures containing these labels was compared against that for pictures with random letter strings or no labels. In Experiment 1, Stroop interference was examined in first, second, and third grade children to determine whether there was developmental change in the presence of rime or GPC interference. In Experiment 2, Stroop interference was examined as a function of relative reading skill in first grade children. In Experiment 3, Stroop interference in adults was compared to the use of rime or GPC pronunciation strategies for nonword reading. In all experiments, Stroop interference in picture naming was longer for pictures with highly predictable GPC unit labels than less predictable GPC unit labels. However, in Experiment 3, even though adults showed interference from predictable GPC units in the Stroop task, they always preferred rime pronunciation for ambiguous nonwords in the nonword reading task. It is argued that the current experiments provide evidence for a flexible units model.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Adams, M. J., Foorman, B. R., Lundberg, I., & Beeler, T. (1998). Phonemic awareness in young children: A classroom curriculum. Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bakan, P., & Alperson, B. (1967). Pronounceability, attensity, and interference in the color-word test. American Journal of Psychology, 80, 416–420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berent, I., & Marom, M. (2005). Skeletal structure of printed words: Evidence from the stroop task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 328–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berndt, R. S., Reggia, J. A., & Mitchum, C. C. (1987). Empirically-derived probabilities for grapheme-to-phoneme correspondences in English. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 19, 1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowey, J. A., & Underwood, N. (1996). Further evidence that orthographic rime usage in nonword reading increases with word-level reading proficiency. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 526–562.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowey, J. A., Vaughan, L., & Hansen, J. (1998). Beginning readers’ use of orthograpahic analogies in word reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 68, 108–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, G. D. A. (1998). The endpoint of skilled word recognition: The ROAR model. In J. L. Metsala & L. C. Ehri (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 121–138). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

  • Brown, G. D. A., & Deavers, R. P. (1999). Units of analysis in nonword reading: Evidence from children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 73, 208–242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruck, M., & Treiman, R. (1992). Learning to pronounce words: The limitations of analogies. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 374–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, P. E., Bradley, L., Maclean, M., & Crossland, J. (1989). Nursery rhymes, phonological skills, and reading. Journal of Child Language, 16, 407–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chall, J. S. (1996). Stages of reading development (2nd ed.). Fort Worth: Harcourt Brace College Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Christensen, C. A., & Bowey, J. A. (2005). The efficacy of orthographic rime, grapheme-phoneme correspondence, and implicit phonics approaches to teaching decoding skills. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 327–349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, V., & Leahy, J. (1992). Children’s and adults’ reading of nonwords: Effects of regularity and consistency. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 718–729.

    Google Scholar 

  • Comalli, P. E., Jr., Wapner, S., & Werner, H. (1962). Interference effects of Stroop color-word test in childhood, adulthood, and aging. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 100, 47–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, P. M., & Cunningham, J. W. (1992). Making words. Enhancing the invented spelling-decoding connection. The Reading Teacher, 46, 106–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cycowicz, Y. M., Friedman, D., Rothstein, M, & Snodgrass, J. G. (1997). Picture naming by young children: Norms for name agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 65, 171–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deavers, R., Solity, J., & Kerfoot, S. (2000). The effect of instruction on early nonword reading strategies. Journal of Research in Reading, 23, 267–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, L. G., Seymour, P. H. K., & Hill, S. (2000). A small-to-large progression in metaphonological awareness and reading? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 53, 1081–1104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. (1976). Do words really interfere in naming pictures? Child Development, 47, 502–505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. (1995). Phases of development in learning to read words by sight. Journal of Research in Reading, 18, 116–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. (1997). Learning to read and learning to spell are one and the same, almost. In C. A. Perfetti, L. Rieben, & M. Fayol (Eds.). Learning to spell: Research, theory, and practice across languages (pp. 237–269). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

  • Ehri, L. C., & Wilce, L. S. (1979). Does word training increase or decrease interference in a Stroop task? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 27, 352–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engelmann, S. (1995). Reading Mastery V. Worthington, OH: SRA Macmillan/McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golinkoff, R. M., & Rosinski, R. R. (1976). Decoding, semantic processing, and reading comprehension skill. Child Development, 47, 252–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gombert, J. E. (1992). Metalinguistic development. London: Harvester Wheatsheaf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U. (1993). Toward an interactive analogy model of reading development: Decoding vowel graphemes in beginning reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 56, 443–475.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U. (1999). Causal connections in beginning reading: The importance of rhyme. Journal of Research in Reading, 22, 217–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U., Ziegler, J. C., Dalton, L, & Schneider, W. (2003). Nonword reading across orthographies: How flexible is the choice of reading units? Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 235–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guttentag, R. E., & Haith, M. M. (1978). Automatic processing as a function of age and reading ability. Child Development, 49, 707–716.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, D., & Samuels, S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 293–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. (1997). Automaticity and reading: Perspectives from the instance theory of automatization. Reading and Writing Quarterly: Overcoming Learning Difficulties, 13, 123–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maclean, M., Bryant, P., & Bradley, L. (1987). Rhymes, nursery rhymes, and reading in early childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 255–281.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manis, F. R., Szeszulski, P. A., Howell, M. J., & Horn, C. C. (1986). A comparison of analogy- and rule-based decoding strategies in normal and dyslexic children. Journal of Reading Behavior, 18, 203–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marmurek, H. H. C., Proctor, C., & Javor, A. (2006). Stroop-like serial position effects in color naming of words and nonwords. Experimental Psychology, 53, 105–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Monsell, S., Taylor, T. J., & Murphy, K. (2001). Naming the color of a word: Is it responses or task sets that compete? Memory & Cognition, 29, 137–151.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perry, C., Ziegler, J. C., & Coltheart, M. (2002). How predictable is spelling? Developing and testing metrics of phoneme-grapheme contingency. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55A, 897–915.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pritchard, V. E., & Neuman, E. (2004). Negative priming effects in children engaged in nonspatial tasks: Evidence of early development of an intact inhibitory mechanism. Developmental Psychology, 40, 191–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., & Posnansky, C. (1978). Stages of processing in word identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 107, 64–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Redding, G. M., & Gerjets, D. A. (1977). Stroop effect: Interference and facilitation with verbal and manual responses. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 45, 11–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosinski, R. R. (1977). Picture-word interference is semantically based. Child Development, 48, 643–647.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schadler, M., & Thissen, D. M. (1981). The development of automatic word recognition and reading skill. Memory and Cognition, 9, 132–141.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seymour, P. H. K., Duncan, L. G., & Bolik, F. M. (1999). Rhymes and phonemes in the common unit task: Replications and implications for beginning reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 22, 113–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stahl, S. A., & Murray, B. A. (1994). Defining phonological awareness and its relationship to early reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 221–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., Cunningham, A. E., & West, R. F. (1981). A longitudinal study of the development of automatic recognition skills in first graders. Journal of Reading Behavior, 13, 57–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stuart, M., & Coltheart, M. (1988). Does reading develop in a sequence of stages? Cognition, 30, 139–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stuart, M., Masterson, J., Dixon, M., & Quinlan, P. (1999). Inferring sublexical correspondence from sight vocabulary: Evidence from 6- and 7-year-olds. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52A, 353–366.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tipper, S. P., Bourque, T. A., Anderson, S. H., & Brehaut, J. C. (1989). Mechanisms of attention: A developmental study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 48, 353–378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgeson, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Test of word reading efficiency. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., Goswami, U., & Bruck, M. (1990). Not all nonwords are alike: Implications for reading development and theory. Memory & Cognition, 13, 357–364.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., Mullennix, J., Bijeljac-Babic, R., & Richmond-Welty, E. D. (1995). The Special role of rimes in the description, use, and acquisition of English orthography. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 107–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., & Zukowski, A. (1991). Levels of phonological awareness. In S. A. Brady & D. P. Shakweiler (Eds.), Phonological processes in literacy: A tribute to Isabelle Y. Liberman (pp. 67–83). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

  • Venezky, R. L. (1970). The structure of English orthography. The Hague, The Netherlands: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walton, P. D., & Walton, L. M. (2002). Beginning reading by teaching in rime analogy: Effects on phonological skills, letter-sound knowledge, working memory, and word-reading strategies. Scientific Studies of Reading, 6, 79–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilkinson, G. S. (1993). The wide range achievement test: Administration manual. Wilmington, DE: Wide Range Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, C. (2002). Orthographic analogies and phonological priming effects. Journal of Research in Reading, 25, 144–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zeno, S. M., Ivens, S. H., Millard, R. T., & Duvvuri, R. (1995). The educator’s word frequency guide. Brewster, NY: Touchstone Applied Science Associates.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Barbara Bradley, Carolyn Groff, Elisabeth Meisinger, Matthew Quirk, Franklin Turner, and Deborah Woo for their assistance in data collection. We thank Yael Cycowicz for providing us the digital versions of the pictures used in this experiment. This project was supported by the Interagency Education Research Initiative, a program of research managed jointly by the National Science Foundation, the Institute of Education Sciences in the U.S. Department of Education, and the National Institute for Child Health and Human Development in the National Institutes of Health (NICHD NIH). Funding for this project was provided by NICHD NIH Grant 7 R01 HD040746-06.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Paula J. Schwanenflugel.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Schwanenflugel, P.J., Morris, R.D., Kuhn, M.R. et al. The influence of reading unit size on the development of Stroop interference in early word decoding. Read Writ 21, 177–203 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-007-9061-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-007-9061-2

Keywords

Navigation