Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Supporting Scientific Experimentation and Reasoning in Young Elementary School Students

  • Published:
Journal of Science Education and Technology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Researchers from multiple perspectives have shown that young students can engage in the scientific reasoning involved in science experimentation. However, there is little research on how well these young students learn in inquiry-based learning environments that focus on using scientific experimentation strategies to learn new scientific information. This work investigates young children’s science concept learning via inquiry-based instruction on the thermodynamics system in a developmentally appropriate, technology-supported learning environment. First- and third-grade students participate in three sets of guided experimentation activities that involve using handheld computers to measure change in temperature given different types of insulation materials. Findings from pre- and post-comparisons show that students at both grade levels are able to learn about the thermodynamics system through engaging in the guided experiment activities. The instruction groups outperformed the control groups on multiple measures of thermodynamics knowledge, and the older children outperform the younger children. Knowledge gains are discussed in the context of mental models of the thermodynamics system that include the individual concepts mentioned above and the relationships between them. This work suggests that young students can benefit from science instruction centered on experimentation activities. It shows the benefits of presenting complex scientific information authentic contexts and the importance of providing the necessary scaffolding for meaningful scientific inquiry and experimentation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Amsel E, Brock S (1996) The development of evidence evaluation skills. Cogn Dev 11:523–550

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bass KM, Baxter GP, Glaser R (2001) Using reflective writing exercises to promote writing-to-learn in science. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American educational research association, Seattle, WA

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen Z, Klahr D (1999) All other things being equal: children’s acquisition of the control of variables strategy. Child Dev 70(5):1098–1120

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark D (2001) New representations of knowledge integration: theories or repertoires of ideas?. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Seattle, WA

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark DB (2006) Longitudinal conceptual change in students’ understanding of thermal equilibrium: an examination of the process of conceptual restructuring. Cogn Instr 24(4):467–563

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Disessa A (1988) Knowledge in pieces. In: Forman G, Purfall PB (eds) Constructivism in the computer age. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ, pp 49–70

    Google Scholar 

  • Duschl RA, Schweingruber HA, Shouse AW (2007) Taking science to school: learning and teaching science in grades K-8/committee on science learning, kindergarten through eighth grade. The National Academies Press, Washington, DC

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentner D, Stevens AL (1983) Mental models. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NG

    Google Scholar 

  • Inhelder B, Piaget J (1958) The growth of logical thinking from childhood to adolescence. Basic, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird PN (1983) Mental models. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Kesidou S, Duit R (1993) Students’ conceptions of the second law of thermodynamics—an interpretive study. J Res Sci Teach 30(1):85–106

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kesidou S, Duit R, Glynn SM (1995) Conceptual development in physics: students’ understanding of heat. In: Glynn SM, Duit R (eds) Learning science in the schools: research reforming practice. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ, pp 179–198

    Google Scholar 

  • Kind PM (2013) Establishing assessment scales using a novel disciplinary rationale for scientific reasoning. J Res Sci Teach 50(5):530–560

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klahr D, Nigam M (2004) The equivalence of learning paths in early science instruction: effects of direct instruction and discovery learning. Psychol Sci 15:661–667

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klahr D, Zimmerman C, Jirout J (2011) Educational interventions to advance children’s scientific thinking. Science 333:971–975

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn D (1997) Constraints or guideposts? Developmental psychology and science education. Rev Educ Res Spring 67(1):141–150

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn D, Phelps E (1982) The development of problem-solving strategies. In: Reese H (ed) Advances in child development and behavior, vol 17. Academic Press, New York, pp 1–44

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn D, Amsel E, O’Loughlin M (1988) The development of scientific thinking skills. Academic Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis EL, Linn MC (1994) Heat energy and temperature concepts of adolescents, adults, and experts: implications for curricular improvements. J Res Sci Teach 31(6):657–677

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Linn MC (2006) The knowledge integration perspective on learning and instruction. In: Sawyer K (ed) The Cambridge handbook of the learning sciences. Cambridge University Press, New York, NY, pp 243–264

    Google Scholar 

  • Linn MC, Hsi S (2000) Computers, teachers, peers: science learning partners. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahweh, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Linn MC, Songer NB (1991) Teaching thermodynamics to middle school students: what are appropriate cognitive demands? J Res Sci Teach 28(10):885–918

    Google Scholar 

  • Linn MC, Songer NB (1993) How do students make sense of science? Merrill-Palmer Q 38(1):47–73

    Google Scholar 

  • National Research Council (2012) A framework for K-12 science education: practices, crosscutting concepts, and core ideas. The National Academies Press, Washington, DC

    Google Scholar 

  • NGSS Lead States (2013) Next generation science standards: for states, by states. The National Academies Press, Washington, DC

    Google Scholar 

  • Quintana C, Reiser BJ, Davis EA, Krajcik J, Fretz E, Duncan R, Kyza E, Edelson D, Soloway E (2004) A scaffolding design framework for software to support science inquiry. J Learn Sci 13(3):337–386

    Google Scholar 

  • Schauble L (1990) Belief revision in children: the role of prior knowledge and strategies for generating evidence. J Exp Child Psychol 49:31–57

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegler RS (1991) Children’s Thinking  Englewood Cliffs. Prentice-Hall, NJ

  • Siegler RS, Liebert RM (1975) Acquisition of formal scientific reasoning by 10- and 13-year-olds: designing a factorial experiment. Dev Psychol 11:401–402

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slone M, Tredoux C, Bokhorst F (1989) The developmental progression of children’s knowledge constructions for heating and cooling. J Genetic Psychol 15(2):255–266

    Google Scholar 

  • Sodian B, Zaitchik D, Carey S (1991) Young children’s differentiation of hypothetical beliefs from evidence. Child Dev 62:753–766

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Triona LM, Klahr D (2003) Point and click or grab and heft: comparing the influence of physical and virtual instructional materials on elementary school students’ ability to design experiments. Cogn Instr 21:149–173

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tschirgi JE (1980) Sensible reasoning: a hypothesis about hypotheses. Child Dev 51:1–10

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Varma K, Ross P, Lawrenz F, Roehrig G, Huffman D, McGuire L, Chen Y, Su S, Jang Y  (2013)  Unpacking the elements of scientific reasoning.  Presented at the National Association for Research in Science Teaching,  Rio Grande, Puerto Rico

  • Vosniadou S, Brewer WF (1992) Mental models of the earth: a study of conceptual change in childhood. Cogn Psychol 24:535–585

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • White B, Frederiksen J (1990) Causal model progressions as a foundation for intelligent learning environments. Artif Intell 24:99–157

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Windschitl M, Thompson J, Braaten M (2008) Beyond the scientific method: model-based inquiry as a new paradigm of preference for school science investigations. Sci Educ 92(5):941–967

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiser M, Carey S (1983) When heat and temperature were one. In: Gentner D, Stevens A (eds) Mental models. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp 267–297

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman C (2000) The development of scientific reasoning skills. Dev Rev 20:99–149

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman C (2007) The development of scientific thinking skills in elementary and middle school. Dev Rev 27:172–223

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Keisha Varma.

Appendix: Interview Questions and Tasks

Appendix: Interview Questions and Tasks

  1. 1.

    If you were going to wrap a cold soda to take to school for lunch, what is the best thing to wrap it in?

    Why would that be good? Or How would it work to keep the soda cold?

  2. 2.

    What would happen if you wrapped a soda in wool?

    Why would that happen?

  3. 3.

    What would happen if you let a cold soda sit out on a table all day while you were at school?

    Why does the soda get warmer? Or How does that happen?

  4. 4.

    “Insulators and Conductors”

Please write the letter of each material under the column where you think it belongs.

 

Good insulator

Good conductor

Metal (M)

  

Styrofoam (S)

  

Wood (Wd)

  

Glass (G)

  

Wool (Wl)

  

Ceramic floor tile (C)

  

Paper (P)

  

Saran Wrap (Sw)

  

Aluminum foil (A)

  
  1. 5.

    “Thermal Equilibrium Question”

The following objects were left in a room overnight. The temperature of the air in the room does not change. Tell me whether you think the temperature of these objects is above room temperature, at room temperature, or below room temperature. Above room temperature would mean that the object is warmer than the room temperature. Room temperature would mean that the object is the same temperature as the room temperature. Below room temperature would mean that the object is colder than the temperature of the room. After you check your response, tell me “Why do you think so?”

(Check one temperature for each object.)

  1. 6.

    A parent has brought five pizzas for your class party, but your teacher says that you can’t eat until you finish your math work. It will take you about 30 min to finish. How are you going to keep the pizzas warm?

    Why would that work?

  2. 7.

    Susan suggests wrapping each pizza in Styrofoam. Todd thinks you should wrap each one in aluminum foil. Which of these ideas do you think will keep the pizzas warmest?

    Why do you think so?

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Varma, K. Supporting Scientific Experimentation and Reasoning in Young Elementary School Students. J Sci Educ Technol 23, 381–397 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10956-013-9470-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10956-013-9470-8

Keywords

Navigation