Skip to main content
Log in

The Influence of Child-Directed Speech on Word Learning and Comprehension

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This paper describes an investigation into the function of child-directed speech (CDS) across development. In the first experiment, 10–21-month-olds were presented with familiar words in CDS and trained on novel words in CDS or adult-directed speech (ADS). All children preferred the matching display for familiar words. However, only older toddlers in the CDS condition preferred the matching display for novel words. In Experiment 2, children 3–6 years of age were presented with a sentence comprehension task in CDS or ADS. Older children performed better overall than younger children with 5- and 6-year-olds performing above chance regardless of speech condition, while 3- and 4-year-olds only performed above chance when the sentences were presented in CDS. These findings provide support for the theory that CDS is most effective at the beginning of acquisition for particular constructions (e.g. vocabulary acquisition, syntactic comprehension) rather than at a particular age or for a particular task.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Andruski, J. E., Kuhl, P. K., & Hayashi, A. (1999). Point vowels in Japanese mothers’ speech to infants and adults. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 105(2), 1095–1096.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benasich, A. A., & Tallal, P. (2002). Infant discrimination of rapid auditory cues predicts later language impairment. Behavioural Brain Research, 136, 31–49.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, L. (1973). One word at a time. The Hague: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Braarud, H. C., & Stormark, K. M. (2008). Prosodic modification and vocal adjustments in mother‘s speech during face-to-face interaction with their two- to four-month-old infants: A double video study. Social Development, 17(4), 1074–1084.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Broesch, T. L., & Bryant, G. A. (2015). Prosody in infant-directed speech is similar across western and traditional cultures. Journal of Cognition and Development, 16(1), 31–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A., & Draper, P. (2003). Accommodative speech and terms of endearment: Elements of a language mode often experienced by older adults. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 41, 15–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Byers-Heinlein, K., Fennell, C. T., & Werker, J. F. (2013). The development of associative learning in monolingual and bilingual infants. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16(1), 198–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chan, A., Meints, K., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Young children’s comprehension of English SVO word order revisited: Testing the same children in act-out and intermodal preferential looking tasks. Cognitive Development, 25, 30–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, R. P., Abraham, J., Berman, S., & Staska, M. (1997). The development of infants’ preference for motherese. Infant Behavior and Development, 20(4), 477–488.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, R. P., & Aslin, R. (1994). Developmental differences in infant attention to the spectral properties of infant-directed speech. Child Development, 65, 1663–1677.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Boer, B., & Kuhl, P. K. (2003). Investigating the role of infant-directed speech with a computer model. Acoustics Research Letters Online, 4, 129–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Englund, K. (2006). Changes in infant directed speech in the first six months. Infant and Child Development, 15(2), 139–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Espy, K. A., Molfese, D. L., & Molfese, V. J. (2004). Development of auditory event-related potentials in young children and relations to word-level reading abilities at age 8 years. Annals of Dyslexia, 54, 9–38.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fernald, A. (1985). Four-month-old infants prefer to listen to motherese. Infant Behavior and Development, 8(2), 181–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fernald, A., & Kuhl, P. (1987). Acoustic determinants of infant preference for motherese speech. Infant Behaviour and Development, 10, 279–293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Golinkoff, R. M., & Alioto, A. (1995). Infant-directed speech facilitates lexical learning in adults hearing Chinese: Implications for language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 22, 703–726.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Golinkoff, R. M., Ma, W., Song, L., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2013). Twenty five years using the intermodal preferential looking paradigm to study language acquisition: What have we learned? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(3), 316–339.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Golinkoff, R. M., Can, D. D., Soderstrom, M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2015). (Baby) talk to me: The social context of infant-directed speech and its effects on early language acquisition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(5), 339–344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., & Shimpi, P. (2004). Syntactic priming in young children. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 182–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kemler Nelson, D. G., & Smith, J. D. (1989). Analytic and holistic processing in reflection-impulsivity and cognitive development. In T. Globerson & T. Zelniker (Eds.), Cognitive style and cognitive development (pp. 116–140). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kempe, V., & Brooks, P. J. (2005). The role of diminutives in the acquisition of Russian gender: Can elements of child-directed speech aid in learning morphology? Language Learning, 55(S1), 139–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kempe, V., Brooks, P. J., Mironova, N., Pershukova, A., & Fedorova, O. (2007). Playing with word endings: Morphological variation in the learning of Russian noun infections. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25(1), 55–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kempe, V., Schaeffler, S., & Thoresen, J. C. (2010). Prosodic disambiguation in child-directed speech. Journal of Memory and Language, 62, 204–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, P. K., Andruski, J. E., Chistovich, I. A., Chistovich, L. A., Kozhevnikova, E. V., Ryskina, V. L., et al. (1997). Cross-language analysis of phonetic units in language addressed to infants. Science, 277(5326), 684–686.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lidz, J., Waxman, S., & Freedman, J. (2003). What infants know about syntax, but couldn’t have leaned: Experimental evidence for syntactic structure at 18 months. Cognition, 89, B65–B73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, H., Kuhl, P., & Tsao, F. (2003). An association between mothers’ speech clarity and infants’ speech discrimination skills. Developmental Science, 6(3), F1–F10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ma, W., Golinkoff, R. M., Houston, D. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2011). Word learning in infant-and adult-directed speech. Language Learning and Development, 7(3), 185–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, A., Schatz, T., Versteegh, M., Miyazawa, K., Mazuka, R., Dupoux, E., et al. (2015). Mothers speak less clearly to infants than to adults: A comprehensive test of the hyperarticulation hypothesis. Psychological Science, 26(3), 341–347.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matychuk, P. (2005). The role of child-directed speech in language acquisition: A case study. Language Sciences, 27(3), 301–379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McRoberts, G. W., McDonough, C., & Lakusta, L. (2009). The role of verbal repetition in the development of infant speech preferences from 4 to 14 months of age. Infancy, 14(2), 162–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Molfese, D. L. (2000). Predicting dyslexia at 8 years of age using neonatal brain responses. Brain and Language, 72(3), 238–245.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Savage, C., Lieven, E., Theakston, A., & Tomasello, M. (2003). Testing the abstractness of children’s linguistic representations: Lexical and structural priming of syntactic constructions in young children. Developmental Science, 6, 557–567.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Segal, J., & Newman, R. S. (2015). Infant preferences for structural and prosodic properties of infant-directed speech in the second year of life. Infancy, 20(3), 339–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shatz, M., & Gelman, R. (1973). The development of communication skills: Modifications in the speech of young children as a function of listener. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 38(5), 1–38.

  • Singh, L., Morgan, J. L., & Best, C. T. (2002). Infants’ listening preferences: Baby talk or happy talk? Infancy, 3(3), 365–394.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singh, L., Nestor, S., Parikh, C., & Yull, A. (2009). Influences of infant-directed speech on early word recognition. Infancy, 14(6), 654–666.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. E. (1977). Mothers’ speech research: From input to interaction. In C. Snow & C. Ferguson (Eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition (pp. 31–49). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thiessen, E. D., Hill, E. A., & Saffran, J. R. (2005). Infant-directed speech facilitates word segmentation. Infancy, 7(1), 53–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2000). Do young children have adult syntactic competence? Cognition, 74(3), 209–253.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, L., Swensen, L. D., & Naigles, L. R. (2009). Children’s early productivity with verbal morphology. Cognitive Development, 24, 223–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yuan, S., & Fisher, C. (2009). Really? She blicked the baby? Two-year-olds learn combinatorial facts about verbs by listening. Psychological Science, 20(5), 619–626.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Zangl, R., & Mills, D. L. (2007). Increased brain activity to infant-directed speech in 6-and 13-month-old infants. Infancy, 11(1), 31–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This study was funded by a Mount Royal University Internal Research Grant.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Cassandra Foursha-Stevenson.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Appendices

Appendix 1

Kalty

figure a

Dakar

figure b

Blicket

figure c

Bimpko

figure d

Appendix 2

Intransitive

figure e

Transitive

figure f

Appendix 3

Familiarization test script

Screen A

Verbal Stimulus

Screen B

Monkey tickles bunny and vice versa

“See tickling? Can you find where the Monkey is tickling the bunny?”

Monkey tickles self, bunny tickles self

Monkey makes bunny nod and vice versa

“Ohh look nods! Can you find where the monkey nods? Can you find where the bunny nods?”

Monkey nods, Bunny nods

Monkey jumps, Bunny jumps

“Ohh wow jumping! Can you find where the Bunny is jumping over the monkey?”

Bunny jumps over Monkey and vice versa

Bunny tickles self, Monkey tickles self

“Ohh look tickles! Can you find where the Bunny tickles herself? Can you find where the Monkey tickles himself?”

Bunny tickles Monkey

Bunny nods, Monkey nods

“See nodding! Can you find where the Monkey is making the Bunny nod?”

Bunny makes the Monkey nod and vice versa

Monkey jumps, Bunny jumps

“Ohh wow jumps! Can you find where the monkey jumps? Can you find where the bunny jumps?”

Bunny jumps over Monkey and vice versa

  1. Bold indicates the correct answer

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Foursha-Stevenson, C., Schembri, T., Nicoladis, E. et al. The Influence of Child-Directed Speech on Word Learning and Comprehension. J Psycholinguist Res 46, 329–343 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-016-9441-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-016-9441-3

Keywords

Navigation