Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Activating Situation Schemas: The Effects of Multiple Thematic Roles on Related Verbs in a Continuous Priming Paradigm

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Extensive evidence has shown that presentation of a word (target) following a related word (prime) results in faster reaction times compared to unrelated words. Two primes preceding a target have been used to examine the effects of multiple influences on a target. Several studies have observed greater, or additive, priming effects of multiple related primes compared to single related primes. The present study aims to eliminate attentional factors that may have contributed to findings in previous studies that used explicitly presented primes and targets. Thus, a continuous priming paradigm where targets are unknown to participants is used with noun-noun-verb triads filling agent, patient, and action roles in situation schemas (tourist, car, rent). Results replicate priming of single nouns preceding related verbs but do not suggest an additive effect for two nouns versus one. The absence of additive priming suggests that attentional processes may have been a factor in previous research.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ahrens K. (2003) Verbal integration: The interaction of participant roles and sentential argument structure. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 32: 497–516

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ahrens K., Swinney D. (1995) Participant roles and the processing of verbs during sentence comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 24: 533–547

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Balota D. A., Black S. R., Cheney M. (1992) Automatic and attentional priming in young and older adults: Reevaluation of the two-process model. Journal of experimental psychology: Human perception and performance 18: 485–502

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Balota D. A., Paul S. T. (1996) Summation of activation: Evidence from multiple primes that converge and diverge within semantic memory. Journal of experimental psychology: Learning, memory, and cognition 22: 827–845

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, C. (1997). MRC psycholinguistic database (Version 2.00). [Machine usable dictionary]. Available from University of Western Australia Web site, http://www.psy.uwa.edu.au/mrcdatabase/uwa_mrc.htm.

  • Coltheart, M. (1981). MRC psycholinguistic database user manual: Version 1. Retrieved February 9, 2011, from http://www.psych.rl.ac.uk/User_Manual_v1_0.html.

  • Edmonds, L. A., & Mizrahi, S. (2011). Online priming of agent and patient thematic roles and related verbs in younger and older adults. Aphasiology, doi:10.1080/02687038.2011.599527. Retrieved December 1, 2011, from http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2011.599527.

  • Ferretti T. R., McRae K., Hatherell A. (2001) Integrating verbs, situation schemas, and thematic role concepts. Journal of Memory and Language 44: 516–547

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Field A. (2009) Discovering statistics using SPSS: Introducing statistical methods (3rd Ed.). SAGE Publications., Ltd., London

    Google Scholar 

  • Francis W. N., Kučera H. (1982) Frequency analysis of English usage: Lexicon and grammar. Houghton Mifflin, Boston

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilhooly K. J., Logie R. H. (1980) Age of acquisition, imagery, concreteness, familiarity and ambiguity measures for 1944 words. Behaviour Research Methods and Instrumentation 12: 395–427

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grondin R., Lupker S. J., McRae K. (2009) Shared features dominate semantic richness effects for concrete concepts. Journal of Memory and Language 60: 1–19

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hare M., Jones M., Thomson C., Kelly S., McRae K. (2009) Activating event knowledge. Cognition 111: 151–167

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holm S. (1979) A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics 6: 65–70

    Google Scholar 

  • Jarvis, B. G. (2006). MediaLab (Version 2006.2.40) [Computer Software]. New York, NY: Empirisoft Corporation.

  • Jones L. L. (2010) Pure mediated priming: A retrospective semantic matching model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 36: 135–146

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Khader P., Scherag A., Streb J., Rösler F. (2003) Differences between noun and verb processing in a minimal phrase context: A semantic priming study using event-related brain potentials. Cognitive Brain Research 17: 293–313

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kučera H., Francis W. N. (1967) Computational analysis of present-day American English. Brown University Press, Providence

    Google Scholar 

  • McRae K., Boisvert S. (1998) Automatic semantic similarity priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 24: 558–572

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McRae K., Ferretti T. R., Amyote L. (1997) Thematic roles as verb-specific concepts. Language and Cognitive Processes 12: 137–176

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McRae K., Hare M., Elman J. L., Ferretti T. (2005) A basis for generating expectancies for verbs from nouns. Memory and Cognition 33: 1174–1184

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNamara T. P., Altarriba J. (1988) Depth of spreading activation revisited: Semantic mediated priming occurs in lexical decisions. Journal of Memory and Language 27: 545–559

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merritt P., DeLosh E. L., McDaniel M. A. (2006) Effects of word frequency on individual-item and serial-order retention: Tests of the order-encoding view. Memory and Cognition 34: 1615–1627

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moss H. E., Ostrin R. K., Tyler L. K., Marslen-Wilson W. D. (1995) Accessing different types of lexical semantic information: Evidence from priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 21: 863–883

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neely J. H. (1991) Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. In: Besner D., Humphreys G. W. (Eds.), Basic processes in reading. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale NJ, pp 264–336

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, D. L., McEvoy, C. L., & Schreiber, T. A. (1998). The University of South Florida word association, rhyme, and word fragment norms. Retrieved January 21, 2011, from http://web.usf.edu/FreeAssociation/.

  • Nickels L. (2002) Therapy for naming disorders: Revisiting, revising, and reviewing. Aphasiology 16: 935–979

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A., Yuille, J. C., & Madigan, S. A. (1968). Concreteness, imagery and meaningfulness values for 925 words. Journal of Experimental Psychology Monograph Supplement, 76 (3, part 2).

  • Perea M., Rosa E. (2002) The effects of associative and semantic priming in the lexical decision task. Psychological Research 66: 180–194

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raczaszek-Leonardi J., Shapiro L. P., Tuller B., Kelso J. A. S. (2008) Activating basic category exemplars in sentence contexts: A dynamic account. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 37: 87–113

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rastle K., Harrington J., Coltheart M. (2002) 358,534 nonwords: The ARC nonword database. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 55: 1339–1362

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sailor K., Brooks P. J., Bruening P. R., Seiger-Gardner L., Guterman M. (2008) Exploring the time course of semantic interference and associative priming in the picture-word interference task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 62: 789–801

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schnur T. T., Schwartz M. F., Brecher A., Hodgson C. (2006) Semantic interference during blocked-cyclic naming: Evidence from aphasia. Journal of Memory and Language 54: 199–227

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Setola P., Reilly R. G. (2005) Words in the brain’s language: An experimental investigation. Brain and Language 94: 251–259

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro L. P., Brookins B., Gordon B., Nagel N. (1991) Verb effects during sentence processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 17: 983–996

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson C. K., Shapiro L. P., Kiran S., Sobecks J. (2003) The role of syntactic complexity in treatment of sentence deficits in agrammatic aphasia: The complexity account of treatment efficacy (CATE). Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 46: 591–607

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Toglia M. P., Battig W. R. (1978) Handbook of semantic word norms. Erlbaum, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Vigliocco G., Vinson D. P., Lewis W., Garrett M. F. (2004) Representing the meanings of object and action words: The featural and unitary semantic space hypothesis. Cognitive Psychology 48: 422–488

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lisa A. Edmonds.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Herlofsky, S.M., Edmonds, L.A. Activating Situation Schemas: The Effects of Multiple Thematic Roles on Related Verbs in a Continuous Priming Paradigm. J Psycholinguist Res 42, 1–19 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-012-9206-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-012-9206-6

Keywords

Navigation