Skip to main content
Log in

Adult Attachment Orientation and Implicit Behavioral Mimicry

  • Brief Report
  • Published:
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We investigated whether adult attachment orientation predicted the extent to which individuals engaged in implicit behavioral mimicry of a confederate presented by video. Results demonstrated that following an attachment threat prime: (1) those low in attachment anxiety and high in attachment avoidance showed less mimicry of face-rubbing gestures than individuals low in both attachment avoidance and attachment anxiety; (2) those high in attachment anxiety and low in attachment avoidance showed less mimicry of face-rubbing gestures than individuals low in both attachment avoidance and attachment anxiety. Importantly, attachment orientation did not predict baseline levels of face-rubbing gesturing, demonstrating that attachment orientation had an effect on mimicry rather than overall behavior expression. Attachment anxiety was positively related to attraction to the confederate such that those higher in attachment anxiety rated the confederate as more attractive. The findings are discussed with reference to both the mimicry and attachment literatures.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Notes

  1. Readers may wonder whether this task was more difficult for those participants not in a relationship. We compared the number of thoughts and feelings generated by those participants in and not in relationships following the imagined attachment threat. There was no significant difference in the number of thoughts and feelings generated following the imagined threat manipulation t (95) = −.67, p > .51. This provides some evidence that the task difficulty did not differ dependent on the participant’s relationship status.

  2. We note that face-rubbing is an adaptor behavior and that this class of behavior has been associated with anxiety. However, to our knowledge adaptor behaviors have not been shown to be expressed more by individuals higher on attachment anxiety and our baseline measure suggests that this is not the case.

  3. No participants reported that they did not believe the video-link interaction was live, or that the other person was not a real participant.

  4. Readers may think that this is a small variance explained by this predictor. However, within moderated multiple regression analysis, Evans (1985) suggests that a statistically significant R 2 change of 1–2 % demonstrates an effect worthy of consideration. The current results meet these criteria and suggest a result worthy of consideration.

  5. Readers may wonder whether relationship status was included as a factor in our regression analysis. We speculatively recomputed the regression analysis including relationship status as a predictor in block 1. Correlations conducted as part of the regression analysis assessed the relationships between the predictor variables (attachment avoidance, attachment anxiety, Attachment anxiety × Attachment avoidance and relationship status) and the outcome variable (mimicry measure of gesturing). As expected there was a significant correlation between Attachment anxiety x Attachment avoidance and mimicry. In addition there was a significant correlation between relationship status and attachment avoidance r (96) = −.319, p = .001, which meant that those participants not in a relationship had higher attachment avoidance scores. This weak correlation provides some question about whether our variables of attachment avoidance and relationship status are independent of each other. Furthermore, from a theoretical standpoint it is not unreasonable to think that higher attachment avoidance would be predictive of not being in a relationship. The regression analysis revealed that relationship status was not a significant predictor of implicit behavioral mimicry (t (92) = −.069, β = −.007, p > .05). Furthermore, Attachment anxiety × Attachment avoidance remained the only significant predictor t (92) = 2.42, β = .24, p < .05 and the Attachment anxiety x Attachment avoidance interaction still explained 3.4 % of the variance (R 2 adj = .034) and the R 2 change for the second block remained at 5.8 % and was still significant (F change (1, 92) = 5.86, p < .05). Taken together these results suggest that the inclusion of relationship status did not improve the predictivity of the model and therefore we have presented the most parsimonious model in the main text.

References

  • Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ashton-James, C., van Baaren, R. B., Chartrand, T. L., Decety, J., & Karremans, J. (2007). Mimicry and me: The impact of mimicry on self-construal. Social Cognition, 25(4), 518–535.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among young adults: A test of a four category model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 226–244. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.61.2.226.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measurement of adult romantic attachment: An integrative overview. In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 46–76). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behaviour link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893–910. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.893.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cheng, C. M., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). Self-monitoring without awareness: Using mimicry as a nonconscious affiliation strategy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(6), 1170–1179. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.6.1170.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Collins, N. L., Guichard, A. C., Ford, M. B., & Feeney, B. C. (2006). Working models of attachment: New developments and emerging themes. In W. S. Rholes & J. A. Simpson (Eds.), Adult attachment: Theory, research, and clinical implications (pp. 196–239). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins, N., & Read, S. J. (1994). Cognitive representations of attachment: The structure and function of working models. In D. Perlman & K. Bartholomew (Eds.), Advances in personal relationships (Vol. 5, pp. 53–90). London: Jessica Kingsley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crisp, R. J., Farrow, C. V., Rosenthal, H. E. S., Walsh, J., Blisset, J., & Penn, M. K. (2009). Interpersonal attachment predicts identification with groups. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 115–122. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2008.09.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emanuel, L. L. (2011). Nonconscious behavioural mimicry: Examining the methods used to produce mimicry and the automatic nature of the effect. (Unpublished PhD thesis). University of Reading, UK.

  • Eng, W., Heimberg, R. G., Hart, T. A., Schneier, F. R., & Liebowitz, M. R. (2001). Attachment in individuals with social anxiety disorder: The relationship among adult attachment styles, social anxiety, and depression. Emotion, 1(4), 356–380. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.1.4.365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, M. G. (1985). A Monte Carlo study of the effects of correlated method variance in moderated multiple regression analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 36(3), 305–323. doi:10.1016/0749-5978(85)90002-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fraley, R. C., & Shaver, P. (1998). Airport separations: A naturalistic study of adult attachment dynamics in separating couples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(5), 1198–1212. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.75.5.1198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gueguen, N., & Martin, A. (2009). Incidental similarity facilitates behavioral mimicry. Social Psychology, 40(2), 88–92. doi:10.1027/1864-9335.40.2.88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, L. (2002). Behavioral mimicry and stigmatization. Social Cognition, 20(1), 18–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaitz, M., Bar-Haim, Y., Lehrer, M., & Grossman, E. (2004). Adult attachment style and interpersonal distance. Attachment & Human Development, 6(3), 285–304. doi:10.1080/14616730412331281520.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karremans, J. C., & Verwijmeren, T. (2008). Mimicking attractive opposite-sex others: The role of romantic relationship status. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(7), 939–950. doi:10.1177/0146167208316693.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lakin, J. L., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). Using nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create affiliation and rapport. Psychological Science, 14(4), 334–339. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.14481.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lakin, J. L., Chartrand, T. L., & Arkin, R. M. (2008). I am too just like you: Nonconscious mimicry as an automatic behavioral response to social exclusions. Psychological Science, 19(8), 816–822. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02162.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mikulincer, M., Birnbaum, G., Woddis, D., & Nachmias, O. (2000). Stress and accessibility of proximity-related thoughts: Exploring the normative and intraindividual components of attachment theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 509–523. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.3.509.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). Attachment in adulthood: Structure, dynamics, and change. London: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, A., & Carnelley, K. B. (2003). Attachment style differences in the processing of attachment-relevant information: Primed style effects on recall, interpersonal expectations, and affect. Personal Relationships, 10, 59–75. doi:10.1111/1475-6811.00036.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, A. C., Carnelley, K. B., & Willis, H. (2012). Repeated priming of attachment security to keep self-views robust under attachment threat. Unpublished raw data, Universities of Bristol, Southampton, and Gloucester & Cheltenham.

  • Stel, M., Blascovich, J., McCall, C., Mastop, J., van Baaren, R. B., & Vonk, R. (2009). Mimicking disliked others: Effects of a priori liking on the mimicry-liking link. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40(5), 867–880. doi:10.1002/ejsp.655.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Baaren, R. B., Fockenberg, D. A., Holland, R. W., Janssen, L., & van Knippenberg, A. (2006). The moody chameleon: The effect of mood on non-conscious mimicry. Social Cognition, 24(4), 426–437. doi:10.1521/soco.2006.24.4.426.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Baaren, R. B., Horgan, T. G., Chartrand, T. L., & Dijkmans, M. (2004). The forest, the trees, and the chameleon: Context dependence and mimicry. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(3), 453–459. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.86.3.453.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vrijsen, J. N., Lange, W.-G., Becker, E. S., & Rink, M. (2010). Socially anxious individuals lack unintentional mimicry. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48, 561–564. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2010.02.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yabar, Y., Johnston, L., Miles, L., & Peace, V. (2006). Implicit behavioral mimicry: Investigating the impact of group membership. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 30(3), 97–113. doi:10.1007/s10919-006-0010-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was funded by a Nuffield Foundation Social Sciences Small grant SGS36596 to Natalie Hall and Abigail Millings. The authors would like to thank Lia Emanuel and Lauren Cousins for their help with data coding.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Natalie R. Hall.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hall, N.R., Millings, A. & Bouças, S.B. Adult Attachment Orientation and Implicit Behavioral Mimicry. J Nonverbal Behav 36, 235–247 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-012-0136-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-012-0136-7

Keywords

Navigation