Skip to main content
Log in

Assessing conceptual knowledge through solving arithmetic word problems

  • Published:
Educational Studies in Mathematics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We propose to assess conceptual knowledge of mathematical notions by having recourse to isomorphic word problems. We assumed that failing to solve isomorphic problems is an indicator of lack of conceptual knowledge. To reach these conclusions, two experiments were conducted among 4th and 5th grade students. In experiment 1, each student had to solve one isomorphic word problem allowing two solving strategies. Results from experiment 1 showed that semantic context promoted a one-sided perspective, constraining the strategies used. The second experiment questioned if the strategy choices by students reflected a lack of procedural knowledge or difficulty in conceptual knowledge. Students solved several problems with the explicit task of proposing two strategies. Results showed that students struggled to identify the conceptual structure of the problems: they changed their strategy depending on semantic contexts, and did not succeed in adopting two encodings for the same problem. This lack of flexibility does not reflect weak procedural knowledge but difficulties in conceptual knowledge. Once conceptual knowledge is characterized as the ability to get over dependence on semantic context, using word problems as assessment tools for conceptual development opens up educational perspectives.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. F-Nb/S-Price problems is an abbreviation for Factor–Number-of-elements/Summands–Price problems. This type of abbreviation will be used for each problem.

References

  • Baroody, A. J. (2003). The development of adaptive expertise and flexibility: The integration of conceptual and procedural knowledge. In A. J. Baroody & A. Dowker (Eds.), The development of arithmetic concepts and skills: Constructing adaptive expertise (pp. 1–34). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baroody, A. J., Feil, Y., & Johnson, A. R. (2007). An alternative reconceptualization of procedural and conceptual knowledge. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 38(2), 115–131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bassok, M., Chase, V. M., & Martin, S. A. (1998). Adding apples and oranges: Alignment of semantic and formal knowledge. Cognitive Psychology, 35(2), 99–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bassok, M., & Olseth, K. L. (1995). Object-based representations: Transfer between cases of continuous and discrete models of change. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(6), 1522–1538.

    Google Scholar 

  • Begolli, K. N., & Richland, L. E. (2016). Teaching mathematics by comparison: Analog visibility as a double-edged sword. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108(2), 194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bell, A., Swan, M., & Taylor, G. (1981). Choice of operation in verbal problems with decimal numbers. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 12(4), 399–420.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bisanz, J., & LeFevre, J. A. (1992). Understanding elementary mathematics. In J. I. D. Campbell (Ed.), The nature and origins of mathematical skills (pp. 113–136). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publisher.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bisanz, J., Watchorn, R. P., Piatt, C., & Sherman, J. (2009). On “understanding” children’s developing use of inversion. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 11(1–2), 10–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brissiaud, R. (1994). Teaching and development: Solving “missing addend” problems using subtraction. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 9(4), 343–365.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brissiaud, R., & Sander, E. (2010). Arithmetic word problem solving: A situation strategy first framework. Developmental Science, 13(1), 92–107.

    Google Scholar 

  • Canobi, K. H. (2009). Concept–procedure interactions in children’s addition and subtraction. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102(2), 131–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Breton, C. (2002). How specific is the relation between executive function and theory of mind? Contributions of inhibitory control and working memory. Infant and Child Development, 11(2), 73–92.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, T. P., Franke, M. L., Jacobs, V. R., Fennema, E., & Empson, S. B. (1998). A longitudinal study of invention and understanding in children’s multidigit addition and subtraction. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 29(1), 3–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, T. P., Franke, M. L., & Levi, L. (2003). Thinking mathematically: Integrating arithmetic and algebra in elementary school. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coquin-Viennot, D., & Moreau, S. (2003). Highlighting the role of the episodic situation model in the solving of arithmetical problems. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 18(3), 267–279.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crooks, N. M., & Alibali, M. W. (2014). Defining and measuring conceptual knowledge in mathematics. Developmental Review, 34(4), 344–377.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Corte, E., Verschaffel, L., & de Win, L. (1985). Influence of rewording verbal problems on children’s problem representations and solutions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 77(4), 460–470.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deák, G. O., & Wiseheart, M. (2015). Cognitive flexibility in young children: General or task-specific capacity? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 138, 31–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ding, M., & Li, X. (2014). Transition from concrete to abstract representations: The distributive property in a Chinese textbook series. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 87(1), 103–121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischbein, E. (1989). Tacit models and mathematical reasoning. For the learning of mathematics, 9(2), 9–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C., Corley, R. P., & Hewitt, J. K. (2008). Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(2), 201–225.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gamo, S., Sander, E., & Richard, J.-F. (2010). Transfer of strategy use by semantic recoding in arithmetic problem solving. Learning and Instruction, 20(5), 400–410.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilmore, C., Keeble, S., Richardson, S., & Cragg, L. (2015). The role of cognitive inhibition in different components of arithmetic. ZDM, 47(5), 771–782.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greer, B. (1997). Modelling reality in mathematics classrooms: The case of word problems. Learning and Instruction, 7(4), 293–307.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gros, H., Sander, E., & Thibaut, J. P. (2019). When masters of abstraction run into a concrete wall: Experts failing arithmetic word problems. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(5), 1738–1746.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gvozdic, K., & Sander, E. (2020). Learning to be an opportunistic word problem solver: Going beyond informal solving strategies. ZDM, 23(141), 1–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hiebert, J., & Lefevre, P. (1986). Conceptual and procedural knowledge in mathematics: An introductory analysis. Conceptual and Procedural Knowledge: The Case of Mathematics, 2, 1–27.

  • Hudson, T. (1983). Correspondences and numerical differences between disjoint sets. Child Development, 54(1), 84–90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Inoue, N. (2005). The realistic reasons behind unrealistic solutions: The role of interpretive activity in word problem solving. Learning and Instruction, 15(1), 69–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W., & Greeno, J. G. (1985). Understanding and solving word arithmetic problems. Psychological Review, 92(1), 109–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, A. S., Beishuizen, M., & Treffers, A. (1998). The empty number line in Dutch second grades: Realistic versus gradual program design. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 29(4), 443–464.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., & Núñez, R. (2000). Where mathematics comes from. How the embodied mind brings mathematics into being. New York, NY: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pintrich, P. R. (2002). The role of metacognitive knowledge in learning, teaching, and assessing. Theory Into Practice, 41(4), 219–225.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prather, R. W., & Alibali, M. W. (2009). The development of arithmetic principle knowledge: How do we know what learners know? Developmental Review, 29(4), 221–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reusser, K. (1990). From text to situation to equation: Cognitive simulation of understanding and solving mathematical word problems. In H. Mandl, E. De Corte, N. Bennet, & H. F. Friedrich (Eds.), Learning and instruction, European research in an international context, Vol. II. New York, NY: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reusser, K., & Stebler, R. (1997). Every word problem has a solution—The social rationality of mathematical modeling in schools. Learning and Instruction, 7(4), 309–327.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richland, L. E., Stigler, J. W., & Holyoak, K. J. (2012). Teaching the conceptual structure of mathematics. Educational Psychologist, 47(3), 189–203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riley, M. S., Greeno, J. G., & Heller, J. I. (1983). Development of children’s problem solving ability in arithmetic. In H. P. Cinsburg (Ed.), The development of mathematical thinking. New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rittle-Johnson, B. (2017). Developing mathematics knowledge. Child Development Perspectives, 11(3), 184–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rittle-Johnson, B., & Schneider, M. (2015). Developing conceptual and procedural knowledge of mathematics. In R. C. Kadosh & A. Dowker (Eds.), Oxford handbook of numerical cognition (pp. 1118–1134). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rittle-Johnson, B., Schneider, M., & Star, J. R. (2015). Not a one-way street: Bidirectional relations between procedural and conceptual knowledge of mathematics. Educational Psychology Review, 27(4), 587–597.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rittle-Johnson, B., & Siegler, R. S. (1998). The relation between conceptual and procedural knowledge in learning mathematics: A review. In Studies in developmental psychology. The development of mathematical skills (pp. 75–110). Hove, UK: Psychology Press Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rittle-Johnson, B., Siegler, R. S., & Alibali, M. W. (2001). Developing conceptual understanding and procedural skill in mathematics: An iterative process. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93(2), 346–362.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rittle-Johnson, B., & Star, J. R. (2007). Does comparing solution methods facilitate conceptual and procedural knowledge? An experimental study on learning to solve equations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(3), 561–574.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, K. M. (2017). The understanding of additive and multiplicative arithmetic concepts. In Acquisition of complex arithmetic skills and higher-order mathematics concepts (pp. 21–46). Elsevier Academic Press.

  • Robinson, K. M., & Dubé, A. K. (2013). Children’s additive concepts: Promoting understanding and the role of inhibition. Learning and Individual Differences, 23, 101–107.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, K. M., & Ninowski, J. E. (2003). Adults’ understanding of inversion concepts: How does performance on addition and subtraction inversion problems compare to performance on multiplication and division inversion problems? Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 57(4), 321–330.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, M., Rittle-Johnson, B., & Star, J. R. (2011). Relations among conceptual knowledge, procedural knowledge, and procedural flexibility in two samples differing in prior knowledge. Developmental Psychology, 47(6), 1525–1538.

    Google Scholar 

  • Selter, C., Prediger, S., Nührenbörger, M., & Hußmann, S. (2012). Taking away and determining the difference—a longitudinal perspective on two models of subtraction and the inverse relation to addition. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 79(3), 389–408.

    Google Scholar 

  • Star, J. R. (2005). Reconceptualizing procedural knowledge. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 36(5), 404–411.

  • Star, J. R., & Newton, K. J. (2009). The nature and development of experts’ strategy flexibility for solving equations. ZDM, 41(5), 557–567.

    Google Scholar 

  • Star, J. R., & Rittle-Johnson, B. (2008). Flexibility in problem solving: The case of equation solving. Learning and Instruction, 18(6), 565–579.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thevenot, C., & Oakhill, J. (2005). The strategic use of alternative representations in arithmetic word problem solving. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 58(7), 1311–1323.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torbeyns, J., Peters, G., De Smedt, B., Ghesquière, P., & Verschaffel, L. (2016). Children's understanding of the addition/subtraction complement principle. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(3), 382–396.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verschaffel, L., Luwel, K., Torbeyns, J., & Van Dooren, W. (2009). Conceptualizing, investigating, and enhancing adaptive expertise in elementary mathematics education. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 24(3), 335–359.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verschaffel, L., Luwel, K., Torbeyns, J., & Van Dooren, W. (2011). Analyzing and Developing Strategy Flexibility in Mathematics Education. In J. Elen, E. Stahl, R. Bromme, & G. Clarebout (Eds.), Links Between Beliefs and Cognitive Flexibility: Lessons Learned (pp. 175–197). Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verschaffel, L., Torbeyns, J., De Smedt, B., Luwel, K., & Van Dooren, W. (2007). Strategy flexibility in children with low achievement in mathematics. Educational and Child Psychology, 24(2), 16–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vicente, S., Orrantia, J., & Verschaffel, L. (2007). Influence of situational and conceptual rewording on word problem solving. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 77(4), 829–848.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, E., & Stern, E. (2016). Consistent advantages of contrasted comparisons: Algebra learning under direct instruction. Learning and Instruction, 41, 41–51.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank all students and teachers for their participation. We are also grateful to the experimenter Vincent Tambourin and the pedagogical advisor Arbya Eichi.

Funding

This research was supported in part by a National Research Agency Grant ANR-06-APPR-015 and in part by a grant from France’s “Programme d’Investissements d’Avenir” (ANRU/DSDP/D14-2780).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Calliste Scheibling-Sève.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Scheibling-Sève, C., Pasquinelli, E. & Sander, E. Assessing conceptual knowledge through solving arithmetic word problems. Educ Stud Math 103, 293–311 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-020-09938-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-020-09938-3

Keywords

Navigation