Skip to main content
Log in

The Power of Successive Relearning: Improving Performance on Course Exams and Long-Term Retention

  • Replication
  • Published:
Educational Psychology Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Practice tests and spaced study are both highly potent for enhancing learning and memory. Combining these two methods under the conditions in which they are most effective (i.e., practice tests that invoke successful retrieval from long-term memory and spacing study across days) yields a promising learning technique referred to as successive relearning. Given the obvious implications of successive relearning for promoting student learning and the voluminous literatures on testing and spacing more generally, surprisingly few studies have evaluated successive relearning, and none have done so in an authentic educational context. The two experiments reported here establish the potency of a successive relearning intervention for enhancing student learning by demonstrating meaningful improvements in course exam performance and on long-term retention tests.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Based on our post-experiment scoring of participants’ responses, the accuracy of the responses that participants judged as completely correct across all RMF trials in all sessions was 89 % (SE = 2). The accuracy of responses participants judged as completely correct in “Experiment 2” was similar (M = 80 %, SE = 2).

  2. Data collection for Experiment 1 occurred during a fall semester and in a spring semester for Experiment 2. Due to differences in the second half of the course schedule for these two semesters due to Spring Break and to some modifications the instructor made to course content later in the semester, we did not implement the full 14-session schedule in Experiment 2.

  3. Based on analysis of the session dates in the program-internal data files, participants showed high levels of compliance with the intended schedule for Sessions 1–6, with an average of 2.4, 4.9, 2.2, 4.7, and 2.2 days (SEs = 0.2-0.4) between consecutive sessions (cf. the intended lags of 2, 5, 2, 5, and 2 days, as shown in Table 3).

References

  • Bahrick, H. P. (1979). Maintenance of knowledge: Questions about memory we forgot to ask. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 108, 296–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bahrick, H. P., & Hall, L. K. (2005). The importance of retrieval failures to long-term retention: A metacognitive explanation of the spacing effect. Journal of Memory and Language, 52, 566–577.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bahrick, H. P., Bahrick, L. E., Bahrick, A. S., & Bahrick, P. E. (1993). Maintenance of foreign language vocabulary and the spacing effect. Psychological Science, 4, 316–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butler, A. C., & Roediger, H. L., III. (2007). Testing improves long-term retention in a simulated classroom setting. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19, 514–527.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, S. K., & DeLosh, E. L. (2006). Impoverished cue support enhances subsequent retention: Support for the elaborative retrieval explanation of the testing effect. Memory & Cognition, 34, 268–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cepeda, N. J., Pashler, H., Vul, E., Wixted, J. T., & Rohrer, D. (2006). Distributed practice in verbal recall tasks: A review and quantitative synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 354–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chan, J. C. K. (2009). When does retrieval induce forgetting and when does it induce facilitation? Implications for retrieval inhibition, testing effect, and text processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 61, 153–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chan, J. C. K. (2010). Long-term effects of testing on the recall of nontested materials. Memory, 18, 49–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corker, K. S., Oswald, F. L., & Donnellan, M. B. (2012). Conscientiousness in the classroom: A process explanation. Journal of Personality, 80, 995–1028.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daniel, D. B., & Broida, J. (2004). Using web-based quizzing to improve exam performance: Lessons learned. Teaching of Psychology, 31, 207–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deslauriers, L., Schelew, E., & Wieman, C. (2011). Improved learning in a large-enrollment physics class. Science, 332, 862–864.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunlosky, J., Rawson, K. A., Marsh, E. J., Nathan, M. J., & Willingham, D. T. (2013). Improving students’ learning with effective learning techniques: Promising directions from cognitive and educational psychology. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 14, 4–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fritz, C. O., Morris, P. E., Bjork, R. A., Gelman, R., & Wickens, T. D. (2000). When further learning fails: Stability and change following repeated presentation of text. British Journal of Psychology, 91, 493–511.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glover, J. A. (1989). The “testing” phenomenon: Not gone but nearly forgotten. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 392–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hartwig, M. K., & Dunlosky, J. D. (2012). Study strategies of college students: Are self-testing and scheduling related to achievement? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 126–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howe, M. J. (1970). Repeated presentation and recall of meaningful prose. Journal of Educational Psychology, 61, 214–219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karpicke, J. D., Butler, A. C., & Roediger, H. L., III. (2009). Metacognitive strategies in student learning: Do students practice retrieval when they study on their own? Memory, 17, 471–479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kay, H. (1955). Learning and retaining verbal material. British Journal of Psychology, 46, 81–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kornell, N. (2009). Optimising learning using flashcards: Spacing is more effective than cramming. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23, 1297–1317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kornell, N., & Bjork, R. A. (2007). The promise and perils of self-regulated study. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 219–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kornell, N., & Son, L. K. (2009). Learners’ choices and beliefs about self-testing. Memory, 17, 493–501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyle, K. B., & Crawford, N. A. (2011). Retrieving essential material at the end of lectures improves performance on statistics exams. Teaching of Psychology, 38, 94–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McDaniel, M. A., Anderson, J. L., Derbish, M. H., & Morrisette, N. (2007). Testing the testing effect in the classroom. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19, 494–513.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McDaniel, M. A., Wildman, K. M., & Anderson, J. L. (2012). Using quizzes to enhance summative-assessment performance in a web-based class: An experimental study. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 1, 18–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, G. (2012). Revisiting truth or triviality: The external validity of research in the psychological laboratory. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 109–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pashler, H., & Harris, C. R. (2012). Is the replicability crisis overblown? Three arguments examined. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 531–536.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pyc, M. A., & Rawson, K. A. (2009). Testing the retrieval effort hypothesis: Does greater difficulty correctly recalling information lead to higher levels of memory? Journal of Memory and Language, 60, 437–447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pyc, M. A., & Rawson, K. A. (2011). Costs and benefits of dropout schedules of test-restudy practice: Implications for student learning. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25, 87–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rawson, K. A., & Dunlosky, J. (2011). Optimizing schedules of retrieval practice for durable and efficient learning: How much is enough? Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 140, 283–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rawson, K. A., & Dunlosky, J. (2012a). Relearning attenuates the benefits and costs of spacing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. doi:10.1037/a0030498.

  • Rawson, K. A., & Dunlosky, J. (2013). Bang for the buck: Supporting durable and efficient student learning. Submitted manuscript.

  • Rawson, K. A., & Dunlosky, J. (2012b). When is practice testing most effective for improving the durability and efficiency of student learning? Educational Psychology Review, 24, 419–435.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L. I. I. I., & Karpicke, J. D. (2006). The power of testing memory: Basic research and implications for educational practice. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 181–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., III, Putnam, A. L., & Smith, M. A. (2011). Ten benefits of testing and their applications to educational practice. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 44, 1–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, A. M., & Gordon, L. T. (2012). A controlled study of clicker-assisted memory enhancement in college classrooms. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26, 635–643.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spitzer, H. F. (1939). Studies in retention. Journal of Educational Psychology, 30, 641–656.

    Google Scholar 

  • Szpunar, K. K., McDermott, K. B., & Roediger, H. L., III. (2008). Testing during study insulates against the buildup of proactive interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 1392–1399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vaughn, K. E., & Rawson, K. A. (2011). Diagnosing criterion level effects on memory: What aspects of memory are enhanced by repeated retrieval? Psychological Science, 22, 1127–1131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vaughn, K.E., Rawson, K. A., & Dunlosky, J. (2013). Bang for the buck: Successive relearning beats initial criterion level. Paper presented at the 85th annual meeting of the Midwestern Psychological Association, Chicago, IL.

  • Vecchione, M., Alessandri, G., Barbaranelli, C., & Caprara, G. (2012). Gender differences in the Big Five personality development: A longitudinal investigation from late adolescence to emerging adulthood. Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 740–746.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wissman, K. T., Rawson, K. A., & Pyc, M. A. (2011). The interim test effect: Testing prior material can facilitate the learning of new material. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 1140–1147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wissman, K. T., Rawson, K. A., & Pyc, M. A. (2012). How and when do students use flashcards? Memory, 20, 568–579.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Katherine A. Rawson.

Additional information

This research was supported by the Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education, through Grant #R305A080316 to Kent State University. Opinions expressed are those of the authors and do not represent views of the Institute or the U.S. Department of Education.

Appendix

Appendix

Table 6

Table 6 Performance on outcome measures in each condition for each group, Experiment 1

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Rawson, K.A., Dunlosky, J. & Sciartelli, S.M. The Power of Successive Relearning: Improving Performance on Course Exams and Long-Term Retention. Educ Psychol Rev 25, 523–548 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-013-9240-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-013-9240-4

Keywords

Navigation