Skip to main content
Log in

Strength of hand preference and dual task performance by common marmosets

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Animal Cognition Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Study of avian and piscine species has shown that animals with stronger lateralization of the brain are able to perform two tasks presented simultaneously better than can animals with weaker lateralization. We investigated whether this might apply also to primates by testing common marmosets to see whether there is a relationship between the strength of hand preference, as an indicator of strength of brain lateralization, and the ability to carry out two tasks simultaneously. A model predator was introduced into the testing room while the marmoset was foraging. Marmosets with stronger hand preferences detected the ‘predator’ after shorter latency than those with weaker hand preferences. Furthermore, the marmosets with stronger hand preferences produced more mobbing (tsik) vocalizations when they reacted to the predators than did those with weaker hand preferences. There was no such association between hand preference and either latency to respond to the predator or mobbing reaction when the marmosets were not foraging at the time the predator was introduced. Hence, strength of lateralization is associated with the ability to perform foraging and predator detection simultaneously. These results are discussed with reference to the evolution of brain lateralization.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Barros M, Boere V, Mello EL, Tomaz C (2002) Reactions to potential predators in captive-born marmosets (Callithrix penicillata). Int J Primatol 23:443–454

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartecki U, Heymann EW (1987) Field observation of snake-mobbing in a group of saddle-back tamarins, Saguinus fuscicollis nigrifrons. Folia Primatol 48:199–202

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bezerra BM, Souto AS (2008) Structure and usage of the vocal repertoire of Callithrix jacchus. Int J Primatol 29:671–701

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braccini S, Caine NG (2009) Hand preference predicts reactions to novel foods and predators in marmosets (Callithrix geoffroyi). J Comp Psychol 123:18–25

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cameron R, Rogers LJ (1999) Hand preference of the common marmoset, problem solving and responses in a novel setting. J Comp Psychol 113:149–157

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen H-C, Kaplan G, Rogers LJ (2008) Contact calls of common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus): influence of age of caller on antiphonal calling and other vocal responses. Am J Primatol 70:1–6

    Google Scholar 

  • Clara E, Tommasi L, Rogers LJ (2008) Social mobbing calls in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus): effects of experience and associated cortisol levels. Anim Cogn 11:349–358

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dadda M, Bisazza A (2006) Does brain asymmetry allow efficient performance of simultaneous tasks? Anim Behav 72:523–529

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dadda M, Zandona E, Agrillo C, Bisazza A (2009) The costs of hemispheric specialization in a fish. Proc R Soc B 276:4399–4407

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dadda M, Koolhaas WH, Domenici P (2010) Behavioural asymmetry affects escape performance in a teleost fish. Biol Lett 6:414–417

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dharmaretnam M, Rogers LJ (2005) Hemispheric specialization and dual processing in strongly versus weakly lateralized chicks. Behav Brain Res 162:62–70

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Epple G (1968) Comparative studies on vocalization in marmoset monkeys (Hapalidae). Folia Primatol 8:1–40

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrari SF, Ferrari MAL (1990) Predator avoidance behaviour in the buffy-headed marmoset, Callitrix flaviceps. Primates 31:323–338

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon DJ, Rogers LJ (2010) Differences in social and vocal behavior between left- and right-handed common mamrosets (Callithrix jacchus). J Comp Psychol 124:402–411

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Güntürkün O, Diekamp B, Manna M, Nottelman F, Prior H, Schwarz A, Skiba M (2000) Asymmetry pays: visual lateralization improves discrimination success in pigeons. Curr Biol 10:1079–1081

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hook MA, Rogers LJ (2000) Development of hand preferences in marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) and effects of aging. J Comp Psychol 114:263–271

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hook MA, Rogers LJ (2008) Visuospatial reaching preferences of common marmosets: an assessment of individual biases across a variety of tasks. J Comp Psychol 122:41–51

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hook-Costigan MA, Rogers LJ (1998a) Lateralized use of the mouth in production of vocalizations by marmosets. Neuropsychologia 36:1265–1273

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hook-Costigan MA, Rogers LJ (1998b) Eye preferences in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus): influence of age, stimulus and hand preference. Laterality 3:109–130

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hopkins WD (2007) The evolution of hemispheric specialization in primates. Special topics in primatology, vol 5. Academic Press/Elsevier, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Hopkins WD, Bennett A (1994) Handedness and approach-avoidance behavior in chimpanzees. J Exp Psych 20:413–418

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hopkins WD, Washburn DA (1994) Do right and left handed monkeys differ on cognitive measures? Behav Neurosci 108:1207–1212

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lippolis G, Bisazza A, Rogers LJ, Vallortigara G (2002) Lateralization of predator avoidance responses in three species of toads. Laterality 7:163–183

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lippolis G, Westman W, McAllan BM, Rogers LJ (2005) Lateralization of escape responses in the striped-faced dunnart, Sminthopsis macroura (Dasyuridae: Marsupalia). Laterality 10:457–470

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald SE, Pang JC, Gibeault S (1994) Marmoset (Callithrix jacchus jacchus) spatial memory in a foraging task: win-stay versus win-shift strategies. J Comp Psychol 108:879–891

    Google Scholar 

  • MacNeilage P, Rogers LJ, Vallortigara G (2009) Origins of the left and right brain. Sci Am 301:60–67

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Magat M, Brown C (2009) Laterality enhances cognition in Australian parrots. Proc Roy Soc B 276:4155–4162

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGrew WC, Marchant LF (1999) Laterality of hand use pays off in foraging success for wild chimpanzees. Primates 40:509–513

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers LJ (2002) Lateralization in vertebrates: its early evolution, general pattern and development. In: Slater PJB, Rosenblatt J, Snowdon C, Roper T (ed) Adv Study Behav 31:107–162

  • Rogers LJ (2008) Development and function of lateralization in the avian brain. Brain Res Bull 76:235–244

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers LJ (2009) Hand and paw preferences in relation to the lateralized brain. Phil Trans Roy Soc Lond B 364:943–954

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers LJ, Andrew RJ (2002) Comparative vertebrate lateralization. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers LJ, Workman L (1989) Light exposure during incubation affects competitive behaviour in domestic chicks. Appl Anim Behav Sci 23:187–198

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers LJ, Zucca P, Vallortigara G (2004) Advantages of having a lateralized brain. Proc Roy Soc Lond B 271:S420–S422

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rylands AB (1981) Preliminary field observation on the marmoset, Callithrix humeralifer intermedius (Hershkovitz, 1977) at Dardanelos, Rio Aripuana, Mato Grosso. Primates 22:46–59

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rylands AB, Faria DS (1993) Habitats, feeding ecology, and home range size in the genus Callithrix. In: Rylands AB (ed) Marmosets and tamarins. Systematics, behaviour and ecology. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 262–272

    Google Scholar 

  • Searcy YM, Caine NG (2003) Hawk calls elicit alarm and defensive reactions in captive Geoffroy’s marmosets (Callithrix geoffroyi). Folia Primatol 74:115–125

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vallortigara G, Rogers LJ (2005) Survival with an asymmetrical brain: advantages and disadvantages of cerebral lateralization. Behav Brain Sci 28:575–633

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vallortigara G, Rogers LJ, Bisaza A (1999) Possible evolutionary origins of cognitive brain lateralization. Brain Res Rev 30:164–175

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Westergaard GC, Chavanne TJ, Houser L, Cleveland A, Snoy PJ, Suomi SJ, Higley JD (2004) Biobehavioral correlates of hand preference in free-ranging female primates. Laterality 9:267–285

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wichman A, Freire R, Rogers LJ (2009) Light exposure during incubation and social and vigilance behaviour in domestic chicks. Laterality 14:381–394

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wright L, Hardie SM, Rodway P (2004) Pause before you respond: handedness influences response style on the Tower of Hanoi task. Laterality 9:133–147

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Züberbuhler K, Jenny D, Bshary R (1999) The predator deterrence function of primate alarm calls. Ethology 105:477–490

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

LJR is grateful to the Australian Research Council for funding to support this research. This research was part of the requirement towards T. Piddington’s Honours degree in Science at the University of New England.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to L. J. Rogers.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Piddington, T., Rogers, L.J. Strength of hand preference and dual task performance by common marmosets. Anim Cogn 16, 127–135 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-012-0562-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-012-0562-2

Keywords

Navigation