Skip to main content
Log in

Latent profiles of executive functioning in healthy young adults: evidence of individual differences in hemispheric asymmetry

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Two competing theoretical models of individual differences in executive functioning (EF) were examined: the Prefrontal Convexity Model and the Hemispheric Asymmetry Model. Neurologically healthy individuals (N = 315; mean age 20.8) completed a modified switching task (MST) and the Attention Network Test (ANT) in a single testing session. Data analysis was conducted in two phases. In the first phase (model identification), latent profile analysis was applied to MST variables measuring the abilities to form, switch, and maintain mental sets under conditions designed to tax left or right hemisphere resources. In the second phase (model validation), participant clusters obtained from the first phase were compared on the ANT. The Model Identification phase yielded a 3-profile solution consistent with the Hemispheric Asymmetry Model. Profile 1 (N = 203) was characterized by average EF performances. Profile 2 (N = 43) revealed a set maintenance weakness under non-verbal conditions. Profile 3 (N = 38) demonstrated weaknesses in cognitive flexibility combined with poor executive performances under verbal conditions. The Model Validation phase confirmed group differences. Profile 1 demonstrated average EF performances. Profile 2 demonstrated distractibility and decreased alertness, consistent with a right hemisphere weakness. Profile 3 demonstrated cognitive rigidity in the absence of external cues, consistent with a left hemisphere weakness. Individual differences in EF appear to follow a Hemispheric Asymmetry Model of EF among neurologically healthy adults. Investigating the relationship between hemispherically mediated executive functions and other individual difference factors known to confer health risk or resilience could inform numerous disciplines within the field of psychology.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adolfsdottir, S., Sorensen, L., & Lundervold, A. J. (2008). The attention network test: a characteristic pattern of deficits in children with ADHD. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 4, 9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Adrover-Roig, D., Sese, A., Barcelo, F., & Palmer, A. (2012). A latent variable approach to executive control in healthy ageing. Brain and Cognition, 78(3), 284–299.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Allport, D. A., Styles, E. A., & Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting intentional set: exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In Attention and performance: Conscious and nonconscious information processing (Vol. 15, pp. 421-452). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

  • Almerigi, J. B., Carbary, T. J., & Harris, L. J. (2002). Most adults show opposite-side biases in the imagined holding of infants and objects. Brain and Cognition, 48(2–3), 258–263.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Asanowicz, D., Marzecova, A., Jaskowski, P., & Wolski, P. (2012). Hemispheric asymmetry in the efficiency of attentional networks. Brain and Cognition, 79, 117–128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D., Chincotta, D., & Adlam, A. (2001). Working memory and the control of action: evidence from task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 641–657.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baker, S. C., Rogers, R. D., Owen, A. M., Frith, C. D., Dolan, R. J., Frackowiak, R. S., et al. (1996). Neural systems engaged by planning: a PET study of the Tower of London task. Neuropsychologia, 34(6), 515–526.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bamdad, M. J., Ryan, L. M., & Warden, D. L. (2003). Functional assessment of executive abilities following traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 17(12), 1011–1020.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barbey, A. K., Colom, R., Solomon, J., Krueger, F., Forbes, C., & Grafman, J. (2012). An integrative architecture for general intelligence and executive function revealed by lesion mapping. Brain, 135, 1154–1164.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, D. J. (2007). Observations on the use of growth mixture models in psychological research. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 42(4), 757–786.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beane, M., & Marrocco, R. T. (2004). Norepinephrine and acetylcholine mediation of the components of reflexive attention: Implications for attention deficit disorders. Progress in Neurobiology, 74(3), 167–181.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bedson, E., & Turnbull, O. H. (2002). Hemispheric asymmetry for global and local processing: language is less important than one might think. Brain and Cognition, 48, 272–277.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beer, J. S., John, O. P., Scabini, D., & Knight, R. T. (2006). Orbitofrontal cortex and social behavior: Integrating self-monitoring and emotion-cognition interactions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(6), 871–879.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berthoz, S., Armony, J. L., Blair, R. J., & Dolan, R. J. (2002). An fMRI study of intentional and unintentional (embarrassing) violations of social norms. Brain, 125(Pt 8), 1696–1708.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blumenfeld, R. S., & Ranganath, C. (2006). Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex promotes long-term memory formation through its role in working memory organization. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26(3), 916–925.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boone, K. B., Ponton, M. O., Gorsuch, R. L., Gonzalez, J. J., & Miller, B. L. (1998). Factor analysis of four measures of prefrontal lobe functioning. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 13(7), 585–595.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brass, M., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2002). The role of the frontal cortex in task preparation. Cerebral Cortex, 12, 908–914.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brazil, I. A., de Bruijn, E. R., Bulten, B. H., von Borries, A. K., van Lankveld, J. J., Buitelaar, J. K., et al. (2009). Early and late components of error monitoring in violent offenders with psychopathy. Biological Psychiatray, 65(2), 137–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burgess, P. W., Alderman, N., Evans, J., Emslie, H., & Wilson, B. A. (1998). The ecological validity of tests of executive function. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4(6), 547–558.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Busch, R. M., McBride, A., Curtiss, G., & Vanderploeg, R. D. (2005). The components of executive functioning in traumatic brain injury. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 27(8), 1022–1032.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bush, G., Luu, P., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(6), 215–222.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cepeda, N. J., Kramer, A. F., & Gonzalez de Sather, J. C. (2001). Changes in executive control across the life span: examination of task-switching performance. Developmental Psychology, 37(5), 715–730.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, G. (1972). Hemispheric differences in a letter classification task. Perception & Psychophysics, 11(2), 139–142.

  • Collette, F., Van der Linden, M., Laureys, S., Delfiore, G., Degueldre, C., Luxen, A., & Salmon, E. (2005). Exploring the unity and diversity of the neural substrates of executive functioning. Human Brain Mapping, 25(4), 409–423.

  • Colvin, M. K., Dunbar, K., & Grafman, J. (2001). The effects of frontal lobe lesions on goal achievement in the water jug task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13(8), 1129–1147.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Connors, C. K. (2000). Continuous performance test ii. Toronto: Multi-Health System.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coull, J. T., Frith, C. D., Buchel, C., & Nobre, A. C. (2000). Orienting attention in time: behavioural and neuroanatomical distinction between exogenous and endogenous shifts. Neuropsychologia, 38(6), 808–819.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coull, J. T., Frith, C. D., Frackowiak, R. S., & Grasby, P. M. (1996). A fronto-parietal network for rapid visual information processing: a PET study of sustained attention and working memory. Neuropsychologia, 34(11), 1085–1095.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • D’Espositio, M., Aguirre, G.K., Zarahn, E., Ballard, D., Shin, R.K., & Lease, J. (1998). Functional MRI studies of spatial and nonspatial working memory. Cognitive Brain Research, 7, 1-13.

  • Davidson, R. J. (1992). Emotion and affective style: hemispheric substrates. Psychological Science, 3(1), 39–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, R. N., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). Cognitive inflexibility among ruminators and nonruminators. Psychological Medicine, 24, 699–711.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doiseau, F., & Isingrini, M. (2005). Updating information in verbal working memory and executive functioning. Psychological Reports, 96(1), 67–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dreher, J. C., & Grafman, J. (2003). Dissociating the roles of the rostral anterior cingulate and the lateral prefrontal cortices in performing two tasks simultaneously or successively. Cerebral Cortex, 13(4), 329–339.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Duffy, J. D., & Campbell, J. J, 3rd. (1994). The regional prefrontal syndromes: a theoretical and clinical overview. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 6(4), 379–387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duffy, J. D., Campbell, J. J, 3rd, Salloway, S. P., & Malloy, P. F. (2001). Regional prefrontal syndromes: a theoretical and clinical overview. In S. P. Salloway, P. F. Malloy, & J. D. Duffy (Eds.), The frontal lobes and neuropsychiatric illness (pp. 113–123). Arlington: American Psychiatric Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durston, S., Davidson, M. C., Tottenham, N., Galvan, A., Spicer, J., Fossella, J. A., & Casey, B. J. (2006). A shift from diffuse to focal cortical activity with development. Developmental Science, 9(1), 1–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Emerson, M. J., & Miyake, A. (2003). The role of inner speech in task switching: a dual-task investigation. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 148–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception and Psychophysics, 16, 143–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eslinger, P. J., Biddle, K., Pennington, B., & Page, R. B. (1999). Cognitive and behavioral development up to 4 years after early right frontal lobe lesion. Developmental Neuropsychology, 15(2), 157–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Fossella, J., Flombaum, J. I., & Posner, M. I. (2005). The activation of attentional networks. Neuroimage, 26(2), 471–479.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(3), 340–347.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fan, J., Wu, Y., Fossella, J. A., & Posner, M. I. (2001). Assessing the heritability of attentional networks. BMC Neuroscience, 2, 14.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Farah, M. J., Gazzaniga, M. S., Holtzman, J. D., & Kosslyn, S. M. (1985). A left hemisphere basis for visual mental imagery? Neuropsychologia, 23(1), 115–118.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fasotti, L., Bremer, J. J. C. B., & Eling, P. A. T. M. (1992). Influence of improved text encoding on arithmetical word problem solving after frontal lobe damage. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation: An International Journal, 2(1), 3–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fernandez-Duque, D., & Posner, M. I. (2001). Brain imaging of attentional networks in normal and pathological states. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23(1), 74–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goel, V., Gold, B., Kapur, S., & Houle, S. (1997). The seats of reason? An imaging study of deductive and inductive reasoning. Neuroreport, 8(5), 1305–1310.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goel, V., Gold, B., Kapur, S., & Houle, S. (1998). Neuroanatomical correlates of human reasoning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(3), 293–302.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, R. S., Axelrod, B. N., Heaton, R. K., & Chelune, G. J. (1996). Latent structure of the WCST with the standardization samples. Assessment, 3(1), 73.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gooding, D. C., Braun, J. G., & Studer, J. A. (2006). Attentional network task performance in patients with schizophrenia-spectrum disorders: Evidence of a specific deficit. Schizophrenia Research, 88(1–3), 169–178.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gootjes, L., Raij, T., Salmelin, R., & Hari, R. (1999). Left-hemisphere dominance for processing of vowels: a whole-scalp neuromagnetic study. NeuroReport, 10(14), 2987–2991.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gopher, D., Armony, L., & Greenshpan, Y. (2000). Switching tasks and attention policies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129(3), 308–339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, D. J., Barnea, A., Herzberg, K., Rassis, A., Neta, M., Raz, A., et al. (2008). Measuring attention in the hemispheres: the lateralized attention network test (LANT). Brain and Cognition, 66(1), 21–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grimshaw, G. M. (1998). Integration and interference in the cerebral hemispheres: relations with hemispheric specialization. Brain and Cognition, 36(2), 108–127.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grinband, J., Savitskaya, J., Wager, T. D., Teichert, T., Ferrera, V. P., & Hirsch, J. (2011). The dorsal medial frontal cortex is sensitive to time on task, not response conflict or error likelihood. Neuroimage, 57(2), 303–311.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gur, R. (1980). Cognitive task effects on hemispheric blood flow in humans: evidence for individual differences in hemispheric activation. Brain and Language, 9(1), 78–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hagemann, D., Naumann, E., Lurken, A., Becker, G., Maier, S., & Bartussek, D. (1999). EEG asymmetry, dispositional mood and personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 27(3), 541–568.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halterman, C. I., Langan, J., Drew, A., Rodriguez, E., Osternig, L. R., Chous, L., & van Donkelaar, P. (2006). Tracking the recovery of visuospatial attention deficits in mild traumatic brain injury. Brain, 129, 747–753.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hardman, E., Gruzelier, J., Cheesman, K., Jones, C., Liddiard, D., Schleichert, H., & Birbaumer, N. (1997). Frontal interhemispheric asymmetry: self regulation and individual differences in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 221(2–3), 117–120.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heaton, R. K., Chelune, G. J., Talley, J. L., Kay, G. G., & Curtiss, G. (1993). Wisconsin card sorting test manual: revised and expanded. Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.

  • Henson, J. M., Reise, S. P., & Kim, K. H. (2007). Detecting mixtures from structural model differences using latent variable mixture modeling: a comparison of relative model fit statistics. Structural Equation Modeling, 14(2), 202–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hornak, J., O’Doherty, J., Bramham, J., Rolls, E. T., Morris, R. G., Bullock, P. R., et al. (2004). Reward-related reversal learning after surgical excisions in orbito-frontal or dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(3), 463–478.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hull, R., Martin, R. C., Beier, M. E., Lane, D., & Hamilton, A. C. (2008). Executive function in older adults: a structural equation modeling approach. Neuropsychology, 22(4), 508–522.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jager, G., & Postma, A. (2003). On the hemispheric specialization for categorical and coordinate spatial relations: a review of the current evidence. Neuropsychologia, 41(4), 504–515.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jersild, A. T. (1927). Mental set and shift. Archives of Psychology, 89, 5–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, K. A., Robertson, I. H., Barry, E., Mulligan, A., Daibhis, A., Daly, M., et al. (2008). Impaired conflict resolution and alerting in children with ADHD: evidence from the attention network task (ANT). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(12), 1339–1347.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Karnath, H.-O., & Kammer, T. (2003). Manifestationen von Frontalhirnschadigungen. In H.-O. Karnath & P. Thier (Eds.), Neuropsychologie (pp. 515–528). Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Keele, S. W., & Rafal, R. (2000). Deficits of task-set in patients with left prefrontal cortex lesions. In S. Monsell & J. S. Driver (Eds.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance (Vol. XVIII, pp. 627–651). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keita, L., & Bedoin, N. (2011). Hemispheric asymmetries in hierarchical stimulus processing are modulated by stimulus categories and their predictability. Laterality, 16, 333–355.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keita, L., Bedoin, N., Burack, J. A., & Lepore, F. (2014). Switching between global and local levels: the level repetition effect and its hemispheric asymmetry. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(252), 1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, J., & Ragozzino, M. E. (2005). The involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex in learning under changing task contingencies. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 83(2), 125–133.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne, M. (1982). Hemispheric specialization and the growth of human understanding. American Psychologist, 37(4), 411–420.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Knecht, S., Deppe, M., Drager, B., Bobe, L., Lohmann, H., Ringelstein, E. B., et al. (2000a). Language lateralization in healthy right-handers. Brain, 123, 74–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Knecht, S., Drager, B., Deppe, M., Bobe, L., Lohmann, H., Ringelstein, E. B., et al. (2000b). Handedness and hemispheric language dominance in healthy humans. Brain, 123, 2512–2518.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S. M., Koenig, O., Barrett, A., Cave, C. B., Tang, J., & Gabrieli, J. D. (1989). Evidence for two types of spatial representations: Hemispheric specialization for categorical and coordinate relations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15(4), 723–735.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kouneiher, F., Charron, S., & Koechlin, E. (2009). Motivation and cognitive control in the human prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 12(7), 939–945.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kray, J., Li, K. Z., & Lindenberger, U. (2002). Age-related changes in task-switching components: the role of task uncertainty. Brain and Cognition, 49(3), 363–381.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kray, J., & Lindenberger, U. (2000). Adult age differences in task switching. Psychology and Aging, 15(1), 126–147.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kringelbach, M. L. (2005). The human orbitofrontal cortex: Linking reward to hedonic experience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(9), 691–702.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kroger, J. K., Sabb, F. W., Fales, C. L., Bookheimer, S. Y., Cohen, M. S., & Holyoak, K. J. (2002). Recruitment of anterior dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in human reasoning: a parametric study of relational complexity. Cerebral Cortex, 12(5), 477–485.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Langdon, D., & Warrington, E. K. (2000). The role of the left hemisphere in verbal and spatial reasoning tasks. Cortex, 36(5), 691–702.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Langner, R., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2013). Sustaining attention to simple tasks: a meta-analytic review of the neural mechanisms of vigilant attention. Psychological Bulletin, 139(4), 870–900.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lanza, S., Flaherty, B. P., & Collins, L. M. (2003). Latent class and latent transition analysis (Vol. 2). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lidstone, J. S. M., Fernyhough, C., Meins, E., & Whitehouse, A. J. O. (2009). Brief report: inner speech impairment in children with autism is associated with greater nonverbal than verbal skills. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(8), 1222–1225.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lo, Y., Mendell, N. R., & Rubin, D. B. (2001). Testing the number of components in a normal mixture. Biometrika, 88, 767–778.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lombardi, W. J., Andreason, P. J., Sirocco, K. Y., Rio, D. E., Gross, R. E., Umhau, J. C., et al. (1999). Wisconsin card sorting test performance following head injury: dorsolateral fronto-striatal circuit activity predicts perseveration. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21(1), 2–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loring, D. W., Meador, K. J., & Lee, G. P. (1989). Differential-handed response to verbal and visual spatial stimuli: evidence of specialized hemispheric processing following callosotomy. Neuropsychologia, 27(6), 811–827.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lorist, M. M., Klein, M., Nieuwenhuis, S., De Jong, R., Mulder, G., & Meijman, T. F. (2000). Mental fatigue and task control: planning and preparation. Psychophysiology, 37(5), 614–625.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lubke, G., & Neale, M. C. (2006). Distinguishing between latent classes and continuous factors: resolution by maximum likelihood? Multivariate Behavioral Research, 41, 499–532.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R., & Tsvetkova, L. S. (1967). Toward the mechanisms of ‘dynamic aphasia’. Acta Neurologica et Psychiatrica Belgica, 67(11), 1045–1057.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald, A. W, 3rd, Cohen, J. D., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2000). Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science, 288(5472), 1835–1838.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mah, L., Arnold, M. C., & Grafman, J. (2004). Impairment of social perception associated with lesions of the prefrontal cortex. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161(7), 1247–1255.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, J. E., Pilgrim, L. K., & Sorqvist, P. (2013). Hemispheric specialization in selective attention and short-term memory: a fine course model of left- and right-ear disadvantages. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(976), 1–6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, A. (1999). Automatic activation of the medial temporal lobe during encoding: lateralized influences of meaning and novelty. Hippocampus, 9(1), 62–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mathia, J. L., Beall, J. A., & Bigler, E. D. (2004). Neuropsychological and information processing deficits following mild traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10(2), 286–297.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGuire, P. K., Silersweig, D. A., Murray, R. M., David, A. S., Frackowiak, R. S., & Frith, C. D. (1996). Functional anatomy of inner speech and auditory verbal imagery. Psychological Medicine, 26(1), 29–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mecklinger, A. D., von Cramon, D. Y., Springer, A., & Matthes-von Cramon, G. (1999). Executive control functions in task switching: evidence from brain injured patients. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21(5), 606–619.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mega, M. S., & Cummings, J. L. (2001). Frontal subcortical circuits: anatomy and function. In S. P. Salloway, P. F. Malloy, & J. D. Duffy (Eds.), The frontal lobes and neuropsychiatric illness (pp. 15–32). Washington D.C.: American Psychiatric Publishing Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 167–202.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Emerson, M. J., Padilla, F., & Ahn, J. C. (2004). Inner speech as a retrieval aid for task goals: the effects of cue type and articulatory suppression in the random task cuing paradigm. Acta Psychologia (Amst.), 115(2–3), 123–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 8–14.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Monsell, S. (2003). Task switching. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(3), 134–140.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Monsell, S., Yeung, N., & Azuma, R. (2000). Reconfiguration of task-set: is it easier to switch to the weaker task? Psychological Research, 63(3–4), 250–264.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morin, A., & Michaud, J. (2007). Self-awareness and the left inferior frontal gyrus: inner speech use during self-related processing. Brain Research Bulletin, 74(6), 387–396.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Muthen, L. K., & Muthen, B. O. (2009). Mplus user’s guide (5th ed.). Los Angeles.

  • Nagahama, Y., Okina, T., Suzuki, N., Matsuzaki, S., Yamauchi, H., Nabatame, H., et al. (2003). Factor structure of a modified version of the wisconsin card sorting test: an analysis of executive deficit in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 16(2), 103–112.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nagel, B. J., Herting, M. M., Maxwell, E. C., Bruno, R., & Fair, D. (2013). Hemispheric lateralization of verbal and spatial working memory during adolescence. Brain and Cognition, 82, 58–68.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Nagin, D. S. (1999). Analyzing developmental trajectories: a semi-parametric group-based approach. Psychological Methods, 4, 139–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nathaniel-James, D. A., & Frith, C. D. (2002). The role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: evidence from the effects of contextual constraint in a sentence completion task. Neuroimage, 16(4), 1094–1102.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Newman, S. D., Carpenter, P. A., Varma, S., & Just, M. A. (2003). Frontal and parietal participation in problem solving in the Tower of London: fMRI and computational modeling of planning and high-level perception. Neuropsychologia, 41(12), 1668–1682.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Newman, J. P., Patterson, C. M., & Kosson, D. S. (1987). Response perseveration in psychopaths. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96(2), 145–148.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Newman, J. P., Schmitt, W. A., & Voss, W. D. (1997). The impact of motivationally neutral cues on psychopathic individuals: assessing the generality of the response modulation hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106(4), 563–575.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nylund, K. L., Asparouhov, T., & Muthen, B. O. (2007). Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: a Monte Carlo simulation study. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 14, 535–569.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oberlin, B. G., Alford, J. L., & Marrocco, R. T. (2005). Normal attention orienting but abnormal stimulus alerting and conflict effect in combined subtype of ADHD. Behavioural Brain Research, 165(1), 1–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ocklenburg, S., Gunturkun, O., & Beste, C. (2011). Lateralized neural mechanisms underlying the modulation of response inhibition processes. NeuroImage, 55, 1771–1778.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ocklenburg, S., Gunturkun, O., & Beste, C. (2012). Hemispheric asymmetries and cognitive flexibility: an ERP and sLORETA study. Brain and Cognition, 78, 148–155.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Osmon, D. C. (1996). Understanding symptoms of medial frontal lobe disorder: a clinical case study. Journal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings, 3(1), 23–39.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Owen, A. M., Stern, C. E., Look, R. B., Tracey, I., Rosen, B. R., & Petrides, M. (1998). Functional organization of spatial and nonspatial working memory processing within the human lateral frontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95(13), 7721–7726.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pare, N., Rabin, L. A., Fogel, J., & Pepin, M. (2009). Mild traumatic brain injury and its sequelae: characterisation of divided attention deficits. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation: An International Journal, 19(1), 110–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paus, T., Collins, D. L., Evans, A. C., Leonard, G., Pike, B., & Zijdenbos, A. (2001). Maturation of white matter in the human brain: a review of magnetic resonance studies. Brain Research Bulletin, 54(3), 255–266.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Phan, K. L., Taylor, S. F., Welsh, R. C., Decker, L. R., Noll, D. C., Nichols, T. E., et al. (2003). Activation of the medial prefrontal cortex and extended amygdala by individual ratings of emotional arousal: a fMRI study. Biological Psychiatry, 53(3), 211–215.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Picton, T. W., Stuss, D. T., Shallice, T., Alexander, M. P., & Gillingham, S. (2006). Keeping time: effects of focal frontal lesions. Neuropsychologia, 44(7), 1195–1209.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Piguet, O., Grayson, D. A., Tate, R. L., Bennett, H. P., Lye, T. C., Creasey, H., et al. (2005). A model of executive functions in very old community dwellers: evidence from The Sydney Older Persons Study. Cortex, 41(1), 27–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pochon, J. B., Levy, R., Poline, J. B., Crozier, S., Lehericy, S., Pillon, B., et al. (2001). The role of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in the preparation of forthcoming actions: an fMRI study. Cerebral Cortex, 11(3), 260–266.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R., & Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 109(2), 160–174.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prabhakaran, V., Narayanan, K., Zhao, Z., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2000). Integration of diverse information in working memory within the frontal lobe. Nature Neuroscience, 3(1), 85–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pujol, J., Vendrell, P., Deus, J., Junque, C., Bello, J., Marti-Vilalta, J. L., et al. (2001). The effect of medial frontal and posterior parietal demyelinating lesions on stroop interference. Neuroimage, 13(1), 68–75.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ridderinkhof, K. R., Ullsperger, M., Crone, E. A., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2004). The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science, 306, 443–447.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, R. D., Sahakian, B. J., Hodges, J. R., Polkey, C. E., Kennard, C., & Robbins, T. W. (1998). Dissociating executive mechanisms of task control following frontal lobe damage and Parkinson’s disease. Brain, 121(Pt 5), 815–842.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rueckert, L., & Grafman, J. (1998). Sustained attention deficits in patients with lesions of posterior cortex. Neuropsychologia, 36(7), 653–660.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rushworth, M. F., Walton, M. E., Kennerley, S. W., & Bannerman, D. M. (2004). Action sets and decisions in the medial frontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(9), 410–417.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sackeim, H. A. (1978). Lateral asymmetry in intensity of emotional expression. Neuropsychologia, 16(4), 473–481.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Savla, G. N., Twamley, E. W., Delis, D. C., Roesch, S. C., Jeste, D. V., & Palmer, B. W. (2012). Dimensions of executive functioning in schizophrenia and their relationship with processing speed. Schizophria Bulletin, 38(4), 760–768.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scherf, K. S., Sweeney, J. A., & Luna, B. (2006). Brain basis of developmental change in visuospatial working memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(7), 1045–1058.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schretlen, D., Pearlson, G. D., Anthony, J. C., Aylward, E. H., Augustine, A. M., Davis, A., et al. (2000). Elucidating the contributions of processing speed, executive ability, and frontal lobe volume to normal age-related differences in fluid intelligence. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6(1), 52–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sclove, L. S. (1987). Application of model-selection criteria to some problems in multivariate analysis. Psychometrika, 52, 333–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shimamura, A. P. (2000). The role of the prefrontal cortex in dynamic filtering. Psychobiology, 28(2), 207–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1997). Working memory: a view from neuroimaging. Cognitive Psychology, 33, 5–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. E., Jonides, J., Koeppe, R. A., Awh, E., Schumacher, E. H., & Minoshima, S. (1995). Spatial versus object working memory: PET investigations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7(3), 337–356.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smithson, M. (2003). Confidence intervals. Sage University Papers Series on Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences, 07–140. Thousand Oaks: Sage.

  • Sokolov, A. N. (1972). Inner speech and thought. New York: Plenum Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Stefanatos, G. A., & Wasserstein, J. (2001). Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder as a right hemisphere syndrome. Selective literature review and detailed neuropsychological case studies. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 931, 172–195.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stelzel, C., Kraft, A., Brandt, S. A., & Schubert, T. (2008). Dissociable neural effects on task order control and task set maintenance during dual-task processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(4), 613–628.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stiles, J., Moses, P., Passarotti, A., Dick, F. K., & Buxton, R. (2003). Exploring developmental change in the neural bases of higher cognitive functions: the promise of functional magnetic resonance imaging. Developmental Neuropsychology, 24(2–3), 641–668.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stock, A.-K., & Beste, C. (2014). Lateralization of spatial information processing in response monitoring. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(22), 1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stock, A.-K., Wascher, E., & Beste, C. (2013). Differential effects of motor efference copies and proprioceptive information on response evaluation processes. PLoS ONE, 8(4), e62335.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Stout, J. C., Ready, R. E., Grace, J., Malloy, P. F., & Paulsen, J. S. (2003). Factor analysis of the frontal systems behavior scale (FrSBe). Assessment, 10(1), 79–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sturm, W., & Willmes, K. (2001). On the functional neuroanatomy of intrinsic and phasic alertness. Neuroimage, 14(1 Pt 2), S76–S84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stuss, D. T., Alexander, M. P., Floden, D., Binns, M. A., Levine, B., McIntosh, A. R., et al. (2002). Fractionation and localization of distinct frontal lobe processes: evidence from focal lesions in humans. In D. T. Stuss & R. T. Knight (Eds.), Principles of frontal lobe function (pp. 392–407). London: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Stuss, D. T., Floden, D., Alexander, M. P., Levine, B., & Katz, D. (2001). Stroop performance in focal lesion patients: dissociation of processes and frontal lobe lesion location. Neuropsychologia, 39, 771–786.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Suchy, Y. (2009). Executive functioning: overview, assessment, and research issues for non-neuropsychologists. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 37(2), 106–116.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Suchy, Y., Gold, A., Biechler, R., & Osmon, D. C. (2003). Set maintenance and switching differences in college students with inattentive and impulsive features. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 18, 765–766.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suchy, Y., & Kosson, D. S. (2006). Forming, switching, and maintaining mental sets among psychopathic offenders during verbal and nonverbal tasks: another look at the left-hemisphere activation hypothesis. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12(4), 538–548.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sylvester, C. Y., Wager, T. D., Lacey, S. C., Hernandez, L., Nichols, T. E., Smith, E. E., et al. (2003). Switching attention and resolving interference: fMRI measures of executive functions. Neuropsychologia, 41(3), 357–370.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Takio, F., Koivisto, M., & Hamalainen, H. (2014). The influence of executive functions on spatial biases varies during the lifespan. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 170–180.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Takio, F., Koivisto, M., Jokiranta, L., Rashind, F., Kallio, J., Tuominen, T., … Hamalainen, H. (2009). The effect of age on attentional modulation in dichotic listening. Developmental Neuropsychology, 34(3), 225–239.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thomason, M. E., Race, E., Burrows, B., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., Glove, H. G., & Babrieli, J. D. E. (2009). Development of spatial and verbal working memory capacity in the human brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(2), 316–332.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tofighi, D., & Enders, C. K. (2007). Identifying the correct number of classes in growth mixture models. In G. R. Hancock & K. M. Samuelsen (Eds.), Advances in latent variable mixture models (pp. 317–341). Greewich: Information Age.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomarken, A. J., & Keener, A. D. (1998). Frontal brain asymmetry and depression: a self-regulatory perspective. Cognition and Emotion, 12(3), 387–420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tucker, D. M., & Williamson, P. A. (1984). Asymmetric neural control systems in human self-regulation. Psychological Review, 91(2), 185–215.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vaughan, L., & Giovanello, K. (2010). Executive function in daily life: age-related influences of executive processes on instrumental activities of daily living. Psychology and Aging, 25(2), 343–355.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vendrell, P., Junque, C., Pujol, J., Jurado, M. A., Molet, J., & Grafman, J. (1995). The role of prefrontal regions in the Stroop task. Neuropsychologia, 33(3), 341–352.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vigneau, M., Beaucousin, V., Herve, P. Y., Duffau, H., Crivello, F., Houde, O., … Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. (2006). Meta-analyzing left hemisphere language areas: phonology, semantics, and sentence processing. NeuroImage, 30(4), 1414–1432.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vuong, Q. H. (1989). Likelihood ratio tests for model selection and non-tested hypotheses. Econometrics, 57, 307–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wallace, G. L., Silvers, J. A., Martin, A., & Kenworthy, L. E. (2009). Brief report: further evidence for inner speech deficits in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 1735–1739.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler adult intelligence scale (4th ed.). San Antonio: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitmer, A. J., & Banich, M. T. (2007). Inhibition versus switching deficits in different forms of rumination. Psychological Science, 18(6), 546–553.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilk, H. A., Ezekiel, F., & Morton, J. B. (2012). Brain regions associated with moment-to-moment adjustments in control and stable task-set maintenance. Neuroimage, 59(2), 1960–1967.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilkins, A., & Stewart, A. (1974). The time course of lateral asymmetries in visual perception of letters. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102(5), 905–908.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, P. G., Suchy, Y., & Rau, H. K. (2009). Individual differences in executive functioning: implications for stress regulation. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 37(2), 126–140.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Willis, S. G., Wheatley, G. H., & Mitchell, O. R. (1979). Cerebral processing of spatial and verbal analytic tasks: an EEG study. Neuropsychologia, 17, 473–484.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Willner, P., Bailey, R., Parry, R., & Dymond, S. (2010). Evaluation of executive functioning in people with intellectual disabilities. Journal of Intellectual and Disability Research, 54(4), 366–379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Holly K. Rau.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Rau, H.K., Suchy, Y., Butner, J.E. et al. Latent profiles of executive functioning in healthy young adults: evidence of individual differences in hemispheric asymmetry. Psychological Research 80, 997–1019 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-015-0706-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-015-0706-5

Keywords

Navigation